IMR Press / JIN / Volume 23 / Issue 3 / DOI: 10.31083/j.jin2303060
Open Access Original Research
Optogenetic Inhibition of the Cortical Efferents to the Locus Ceruleus Region of Pontine Tegmentum Causes Cognitive Deficits
Show Less
1 Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Chicago Medical School, Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science, North Chicago, IL 60064, USA
*Correspondence: eugene.dimitrov@rosalindfranklin.edu (Eugene Dimitrov)
J. Integr. Neurosci. 2024, 23(3), 60; https://doi.org/10.31083/j.jin2303060
Submitted: 29 September 2023 | Revised: 15 November 2023 | Accepted: 27 November 2023 | Published: 19 March 2024
Copyright: © 2024 The Author(s). Published by IMR Press.
This is an open access article under the CC BY 4.0 license.
Abstract

Background: The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is synaptically coupled to locus ceruleus (LC) located in the pontine tegmentum. The LC supplies norepinephrine (NE) to most of the central nervous system (CNS) via an elaborate efferent network. NE release in the cortex and various limbic structures regulates arousal, memory processes, adaptive behavior and cognitive control. Methods: The study investigated the role of the mPFC-LC circuit in the cognitive behavior of mice. The mPFC efferents were inhibited optogenetically at the level of dorso-rostral pons by virally delivered ArchT opsin. The mice were implanted bilaterally with optic fibers transmitting yellow light and tested for anxiety-like behavior on Elevated O-maze (EOM), for long-term memory with Novel Object Recognition test (NOR), for problem-solving ability with Puzzle test and for learning with Cued Fear Conditioning (FC). In addition, we used anterograde transsynaptic viral tracing to map a possible anatomical circuit allowing the mPFC to modulate the activity of LC neurons, which supply NE to the main limbic structures with a functional role in cognitive behavior. Results: The application of yellow light did not affect the anxiety-like behavior of the mice but impaired their ability to recognize a novel object and solve a problem. Optogenetic inhibition of mPFC to LC, in either acquisition or recall phase of FC similarly decreased freezing. The viral tracing identified the following tripartite circuits: mPFC-LC-dentate gyrus of the hippocampus (DG), mPFC-LC-amygdala (Amy), and mPFC-LC-mPFC. Conclusions: Our results reveal essential long-range regulatory circuits from the mPFC to LC and from LC to the limbic system that serves to optimize cognitive performance.

Keywords
medial prefrontal cortex
locus ceruleus
cognition
optogenetics
tripartite neuronal circuit
1. Introduction

The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) projections can have excitatory and inhibitory effects on locus ceruleus (LC) neurons [1, 2], making direct synaptic contact not only with LC neurons but also with the neurons in the surrounding peri-LC dendritic area [3]. The presumptive functional role of the mPFC projections to LC is regulation of both the tonic and phasic noradrenergic activities, where the first is responsible for disengagement from a current task and the last for optimization of neuronal activity during a specific task [4]. The LC is the principal source of norepinephrine (NE) to the central nervous system (CNS) and is comprised of distinct populations of cells that are highly integrated with one another, and each LC sub-region receives region-biased input. The sub-regions of LC can independently activate and release NE in distinct brain regions [5, 6]. An example of this heterogeneous activity can be seen with activation of LC projections to the amygdala (Amy) during fear learning but LC projections to the mPFC during fear extinction [7].

Memories for aversive events are resilient and persist over time [8]. Cued fear conditioning is a standard laboratory paradigm for studying the acquisition, consolidation, recall, and extinction of fear memories. The test relies on the association of an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US), such as mild electric shock and a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS), such as tone. Over pairings, the CS acquires aversive value and creates a memory that triggers freezing in rodents during subsequent CS alone presentations, for review see [9]. The brain circuits engaged in associative process are well established and include auditory and nociceptive cortical and thalamic areas that project to the Amy, where the presentation of the two sensory inputs causes long-term potentiation (LTP) [10] and accelerates synaptogenesis [11, 12]. Both LTP and accelerated synaptogenesis are enhanced by glucocorticoids and NE [13, 14]. Within the Amy, the role of NE in memory consolidation is executed via the activation of β receptors, activation of which promotes both LTP and synaptogenesis [15, 16]. Arousing painful stimuli such as tail pinch and electric shock increases the release of NE in the Amy [17, 18], which enhances the persistence of fear memories [14]. The increased NE release in Amy is dubbed ‘emotional enhancement of memories’, which can be found cross species in both rodents and humans [19, 20]. In addition to the Amy, the context of the fear association is encoded by neuronal ensembles in the dentate gyrus (DG) of the dorsal hippocampus (Hipp), which is essential for the disambiguation of the environmental cues [21]. While early fear engrams are made in Amy and Hipp, long-term aversive memories are also stored in the mPFC [22]. Therefore, the activity of LC is central for cognitive processes in mPFC, Amy, and Hipp brain structures.

While the importance of NE from the LC for optimal performance of various cognitive tasks is well described, the exact inputs responsible for adjusting LC activity for specific cognitive tasks do not have the same level of clarity. The mPFC sends robust projections to the LC, and activation of these mPFC projections can serve as a strong modulatory input in the LC. Yet, little is known about the role of these projections in learning and memory. The present studies use a detailed circuit-based approach to determine the participation of the mPFC projections to the LC in various cognitive activities.

2. Methods
2.1 Animals

Adult outbred male mice, CD1 (cat. 022, Charles River, Wilmington, MA, USA), 8 to 10 weeks old and average weight of 30 grams were used in the study. The animals were housed in conventional cages in an AAALAC-accredited animal care facility and kept at 12:12 h light/dark cycle with food and water available ad libitum. All animal work complies with the guidelines set by the IACUC at Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science, protocol number T23-10.

2.2 Behavior Tests

All behavior tests were done with mice implanted with optogenetic fibers attached to a light transmitter. The light transmitter was off in the control groups and switched on in the experimental groups, delivering yellow light. Behavioral tests were each separated by three to five days. The mice were randomly assigned to groups for different tests; therefore, each mouse had an equal chance to be in the control or experimental group.

2.2.1 Elevated O-maze (EOM)

Elevated O-maze (EOM) is a continuous circular track (approximately 10 cm wide and 50 cm above the floor) with two open areas and two enclosed areas of the track (each 50 cm × 10 cm). Mice were placed in the maze for 5 minutes, and the amount of time spent in each arm was videorecorded. The yellow light transmitter was off in the control group and was on for the entire time in the experimental group. Arm entries are defined as placing all four paws within an arm while the distance traveled was used as an index of motor activity. The recorded sessions were analyzed by an automatic video-tracking system (Any-Maze, Stoelting, Wooddale, IL, USA).

2.2.2 Novel Object Recognition Test (NOR)

The mice were habituated to the testing arena (open field box for rats) in two 5-minute sessions. During testing, the optogenetic light emitters were attached to the mice fiber optic cannula implants, and mice were placed in the testing arena for five minutes with two objects similar in texture and size but not in color. Objects were smaller than the mice. Objects were impermeable plastic labware (e.g., test tube holders, caps, etc.) that were cleaned and sanitized between each testing session. Mice were returned to their home cage for 3 hours and then placed in the testing arena with one original and one novel object to quantify recognition memory. The light transmitters were turned on in the experimental group but remained off in the control animals. The session was videorecorded and the amount of time investigating each object (the mouse nose is less than 2 centimeters from the object) was automatically measured by Any-Maze tracking software (Stoelting Inc., Wooddale, IL, USA). A discrimination index (DI) expressed as a percent was used to estimate memory retention (DI = (TimeNovel – TimeFamiliar)/(TimeNovel + TimeFamiliar) × 100) [23].

2.2.3 Evaluation of Problem-Solving Ability (Puzzle Test)

The Puzzle Test evaluates goal-oriented behavior, relying on natural rodent avoidance of open, bright spaces [24, 25, 26]. We used a simplified protocol based on the Puzzle paradigm, combining a five-minute habituation phase followed by a problem-solving phase with a single obstacle [3]. The test was conducted in a modified mouse light-dark box where the dark and the light compartments were connected with a tunnel made of a plastic tube (15 cm long and 7 cm in diameter). The tube size was chosen to accommodate the headgear of the animals. The mice were left to explore the compartments for five minutes. Next, mice were briefly removed from the box and a barrier made of paper, which the mice could easily pull or push out of the tunnel, was used to block the entrance to the tunnel [26]. The mice were returned to the open, bright compartment, and the time to remove the barrier and enter the dark chamber was measured. The light transmitters of the experimental group were on during the problem-solving phase of the test.

2.2.4 Pavlovian (Cued) Fear Conditioning (FC)

The animals were placed in a commercial conditioning apparatus designed for mice (Any-Maze, Stoelting Inc., Wooddale, IL, USA). The apparatus is a Plexiglas container with metal bars making up the floor, a speaker in the wall, a camera mounted to the ceiling, and changeable walls in different patterns. Mice were habituated to the test box (Box A) in three separate sessions, each lasting 180 seconds. Test Box B was placed in a different room with different wall patterns, grid floor, and cleaning solution for the fear conditioning phase. After three minutes in Box B, the mice were exposed to an 80 dB, 30-second tone immediately followed by a single, two-second 1.5 mA electric shock. The animals remained in the box for another 2 minutes. Seven days later, the mice were tested for fear recall in Box A, where the conditioned stimulus (CS) with duration 30 seconds was presented three minutes after the mice entered the box. The mice were left in the box for an additional five minutes. Any-Maze tracking software (Stoelting Inc., Wooddale, IL, USA) quantified the animals’ freezing as a percent of the total time during the conditioning and testing sessions.

Two fear conditioning (FC) tests were performed using different groups of mice. In the first test, the yellow light was on during the acquisition phase (Box B) and off during the recall phase (Box A). In the second FC test, the light was off during the acquisition (Box B) and turned on in the recall phase (Box A). The control groups had mounted headgear, but the light transmitters were off for both phases of the experiment.

2.3 Stereotaxic Surgeries

Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane (3% induction and 1–2% for maintenance, catalog number 792632, Millipore-Sigma, Burlington, MA, USA). After confirming anesthesia by tail pinch, the mice were placed in the stereotactic frame (Stoelting Inc., Wood Dale, IL, USA) and the skull was exposed via a midline incision. A 10 µL Hamilton syringe with a stainless-steel 32-gauge needle was lowered into the region of interest through an opening drilled through the skull according to the respective coordinates from bregma (LC: anteroposterior (AP) – 5.4, lateral ± 0.8 and dorsoventral (DV) – 3.5 mm; mPFC: AP + 1.8, lateral ± 0.4 and DV – 2 mm; Amy: AP – 1.8, lateral – 3.0 and DV – 4.5 mm; dorsal DG: AP – 2.1, lateral – 1.4 and DV – 2.1 mm). A total volume of 100 to 200 nL of the virus at rate 1 nL/sec, was injected with an infusion pump with the needle in place for 5 minutes. A similar stereotaxic technique was used for placing the optic fibers for optogenetic testing. The fibers were secured to the skull with bone screws and dental cement. The animals were given analgesia (flunixin, 2 mg/kg subcutaneously, Norbrook Inc., Overland Park, KS, USA) for three days after surgery. The mice remained undisturbed for one to three weeks after the surgeries.

2.4 Viruses

For optogenetic silencing of the mPFC-LC pathway, we used an AAV viral construct (pAAV1-CamKII-ArchT-GFP, titer 1 × 1013, cat. number 99039, Addgene, Watertown, MA, USA) expressing the inhibitory opsin ArchT [27, 28], injected bilaterally into the mPFC (200 nL; AP + 1.8, lateral ± 0.4 and DV – 2.0 mm to bregma).

The same stereotaxic procedure was used to trace the tripartite mPFC – LC – limbic regions circuit. Five mice of each group were injected with AAV1-hSyn-Cre-WPRE-hGH (Addgene, 105553) into the right mPFC, with cre-dependent AAV9-pEF1a-DIO-FLPo (Addgene, 87306) into the right LC and with flip-dependent AAV8-pEf1a-fDIO-GCaMP6s (Addgene, 105714) into the Amy in one group, DG in a second group, and mPFC in a third group. The mice were euthanized 21 days later and brains were sectioned for analysis. Mice with misplaced injections or lack of signal were excluded from further analysis. The GCaMP6 (GFP) labeled cells were counted on three to four non-consecutive sections throughout the rostro-caudal extent of the respective structures and averaged per animal.

2.5 Optogenetics

Three weeks after the injection of AAV-ArchT, mice were implanted in the LC with a fiber optic cannula, bilateral, 250 µm diameter, 4.5 mm long, 6.2 mm distance center to center, emitting yellow LED light (590 nm wavelength; Elcom, San Diego, CA, USA). After recovering from surgery, the mice underwent three habituation sessions with headgear (dummy or functional receivers; Wireless receivers, TeleR-1-P, Teleopto, Amuza Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). ArchT is an inhibitory opsin-driven proton pump [28] that requires continuous illumination with low-intensity light [29]. Therefore, to prevent overheating and avoid tissue damage, we set up our pulse generator (STOmk-2, Amuza Inc., San Diego, CA, USA) to 10 ms light pulses with 5 Hz frequency and adjusted the light intensity to 15 mW at the cannula tip as measured by a light power meter, LPM-100 (Amuza Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). Mice with attached headgear were placed in the respective test equipment and the pulse generator was turned on. In the experimental groups, the light emission continued for the entire duration of the tests or 5 minutes. In control groups, the headgear was attached but the light transmitter switched off. The mice were perfused after the completion of the experiments to verify fiber optic cannula placements.

2.6 Brain Tissue Processing

The mice were anesthetized by an injection of sodium pentobarbital (40 mg/kg i.p., catalog number VED111, Penn Veterinary Supply, Inc., Lancaster, PA, USA). After confirming mice no longer responded to tail pinch, mice were transcardially perfused with 20 mL of phosphate-buffered saline (phosphate-buffered saline, PBS: 10 mM Na2HPO4, 150 mM NaCl, pH 7.5) containing 0.1% procaine and 100 U/mL heparin followed by 20 mL of 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS as a fixative solution. The dissected brains were kept in the fixative at 4 °C overnight. The viral expression was evaluated by GFP fluorescence. The LC cells were visualized by immunostaining with sheep anti-TH antibody (1:2000; #2027, Phosphosolutions, Aurora, CO, USA). The brain sections were incubated with the primary antibody overnight, followed by incubation with a secondary antibody solution (1:400; Alexa 594 anti-sheep, Jackson ImmunoResearch Inc. West Grove, PA, USA) for 4 hours at room temperature. Brain sections were then mounted and coversliped with medium to prevent photobleaching during imaging. Leica HD 5500 epifluorescent microscope equipped with a digital CMOS camera (Orca Fusion, Hamamatsu, Japan) was used for imaging.

2.7 Statistical Analysis

Graph Prism 9 (GraphPad Dotmatics, Boston, MA, USA) software was used for all statistical analysis. T-tests were applied to compare the behavior experiments with the exception of FC experiments, where the freezing response during training and testing phases was analyzed with a Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by post hoc Tukey’s multiple comparison test with α set at 0.05. The graphic presentations of the results include individual data points (n).

3. Results
3.1 Optogenetic Inhibition of the mPFC Projections to the LC Area of Pontine Tegmentum Alters Cognition in Mice

The mPFC regulates visceral and autonomic responses via activation of robust projections to the hypothalamus and brainstem centers, including the LC [30]. The role of LC-NE in general arousal, attention, learning, memory, and stress is well established. Here, we tested if inhibition of the mPFC projections to LC would impact similar behaviors. The mice were injected bilaterally into the mPFC with AAV-ArchT-GFP, an inhibitory proton pump, and were implanted with fiber-optic cannula capable of delivering yellow light to the LC (Fig. 1, Ref. [31]). Mice were first tested for anxiety-like behavior in the Elevated O-maze. There was no difference between control (light off; 28.1 ± 6.3%) and experimental (light on; 22.2 ± 5.8%) groups for time spent in the open compartment of the maze (T-test, t19 = 0.7, p < 0.5; Fig. 2A). Next, the necessity of the mPFC-LC projections to nonaversive memory was quantified with novel object recognition memory. The experimental (light on) group spent much less time investigating the novel object, with a DI of 0.5 ± 3.1%, relative to the for the control group (light off), with a DI of 28.4 ± 9.9% (T-test, t13 = 2.5, p < 0.01, Fig. 2B). Next, the Puzzle test was used to assess the necessity of mPFC-LC projections to problem solving ability. Here again, experimental groups (light on, 421 ± 59 sec latency average) took longer to cross a tunnel to enter the dark compartment when compared to the controls group (light off, 252 ± 36 sec latency average; T-test, t21 = 2.4, p < 0.05; Fig. 2C).

Fig. 1.

mPFC efferents cover the pontine tegmentum, including LC proper and peri-LC area. (A) The mPFC efferents expressing ArchT (green) traverse LC (red) and adjacent dendritic regions on both sides. There is minimal expression in the lateral and ventral parts of the pons. Arrows indicate fiberoptic tracks. (B) Schematic representation of the placement of the optic fibers. Behavioral data was collected from twenty-three mice with fiber optic cannula tracks positioned in the yellow areas. Seven animals with placements at approximately the blue X marks were excluded from analysis. LC proper is highlighted in red. The Nissl-stained coronal brain section is adapted from “Allen Brain Atlas”, (http://atlas.brain-map.org/atlas?atlas=1&plate=100960184). Abbreviations: 4th, Fourth ventricle; Br, Barrington nucleus; CG, Central Gray; DTg, Dorsal Tegmental nucleus; LC, Locus Ceruleus; mlf, medial longitudinal fasciculus; PB, parabrachial nucleus; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex.

Fig. 2.

mPFC-LC inhibition did not affect anxiety-like behavior but impaired recognition memory and problem-solving behavior. (A) The control group (light off) and the experimental group (light on) spent a similar amount of time in the open compartments of the EOM, T-test, p < 0.05. (B) The experimental group spent similar time investigating the novel and familiar object at test relative to the control group (T-test, p < 0.05), which indicates a lack of novel object discrimination. (C) The experimental group also displayed an increased latency to remove a barrier and enter the dark compartment in the Puzzle test relative to the control group (T-test, p < 0.05), an indication for impaired problem-solving ability. * p < 0.05. EOM, Elevated O-maze.

3.2 Optogenetic Inhibition of the mPFC Projections in the LC Area of Pontine Tegmentum Decreases CS-Elicited Freezing of Mice

To further investigate the role of the mPFC projections to LC in cognition, we optogenetically inhibited mPFC terminals in the LC area during the acquisition of Pavlovian FC. A week after implantation of the bilateral optic fibers, mice in the experimental group underwent FC with the yellow light emitter on during the presentation of the CS-US pairing presentation (Box B). The control group underwent the same treatment in the absence of yellow light. Seven days later, both groups were placed in a distinct context (Box A) and freezing time was assessed between groups after presentation of the CS. Both groups spent a similar amount of time immobile during the acquisition period of the test with the control group (light off; freezing 12 ± 3.9%) relative to the experimental group (light on; freezing 15.6 ± 5.8%). However, when exposed to the CS, the experimental group spent much less time freezing (27.1 ± 6.9%) than the control group (45.2 ± 4.6%). Two-way ANOVA analysis showed a significant main effect for the Fear factor (F1,24 = 30, p < 0.001), and an Interaction (F1,24 = 7.4, p < 0.05), but there was no main effect for Treatment (F1,24 = 3.1, p > 0.05). The post-hoc comparison between the two groups showed a significant reduction in CS-elicited freezing during recall with mPFC-LC inhibition during FC relative to the control condition (q = 4.5, p < 0.05; Fig. 3A).

Fig. 3.

mPFC-LC inhibition during acquisition or during fear recall similarly reduces CS-elicited freezing. (A) The experimental group (yellow light on) spent less time freezing in response to the CS than the control group (light off) with mPFC-LC inhibition during the CS-US pairing at conditioning. The orange column indicates light exposure, Two-way ANOVA, significant for Fear factor, p < 0.001 and Interaction of Fear versus Treatment, p < 0.05; post-hoc, a significant difference between control and experimental group, * p < 0.05. (B) Previously conditioned mice in the experimental group spent less time freezing when compared to the control group during FC testing (yellow light on). The orange column indicates light exposure, Two-way ANOVA, significant for Fear factor, p < 0.001, Treatment p < 0.01 and Interaction of Fear versus Treatment, p < 0.05; post-hoc, significant difference between off and on group, ** p < 0.01. ANOVA, analysis of variance; CS, conditioned stimulus; US, unconditioned stimulus.

In another group, the mPFC-LC pathway was manipulated during the testing session seven days after FC. Analogous to the previous results, both groups spent a similar percentage of time freezing during the presentation of the CS and US (control group freezing 8.9 ± 2.9%, experimental group freezing 6.3 ± 1.8% average). However, during the recall session, the experimental group (light on, 14.4 ± 4.8% average CS freezing) spent significantly less time freezing than the control group (light off, 38.3 ± 5.4% average CS freezing). Two-way ANOVA analysis showed a significant main effect for Fear factor (F1,24 = 19.6, p < 0.001), a main effect for Treatment (F1,24 = 9.9, p < 0.01), and an Interaction (F1,24 = 6.3, p < 0.05). The post hoc comparison revealed significant difference during CS-elicited freezing with mPFC-LC pathway inhibition (q = 5.6, p < 0.01; Fig. 3B).

3.3 The mPFC Efferents Innervate LC Neurons that Supply NE to the Amy, DG and mPFC

The LC is composed of discrete neuronal subpopulations that deliver NE to distinct brain regions [7, 32]. In our last experiment, we used anterograde transsynaptic tagging to trace the anatomical pathways [33] from mPFC to limbic structures that are functionally involved in cognition. AAV1-cre virus initiated the tracing followed by cre-dependent and flip-dependent viral constructs injected into the LC and DG, Amy and mPFC respectively. This mapped the tripartite circuit of synaptically connected neurons from mPFC to LC and from LC to either the DG, Amy, or mPFC. Separate groups of mice were stereotaxically injected with AAV1-hSyn-Cre-WPRE-hGH into the mPFC, cre-dependent AAV9-pEF1a-DIO-FLPo into the LC and flip-dependent AAV8-pEf1a-fDIO-GCaMP6s into the DG, Amy, or mPFC. The experimental design requires the cre to be postsynaptically transferred from the mPFC projections to LC, where it will activate the expression of FLPo in LC cells. The second-order neurons that connect LC to DG, Amy, or mPFC will deliver FLPo transynaptically to the 3rd order neurons, which are transfected with the flip-dependent virus AAV-GCaMP6s. The 3rd-order neurons that receive direct synaptic input from LC will express GFP; hence, the efferent end of the circuitry will be visualized by the GFP signal in the DG, Amy, and mPFC (Fig. 4). The analysis of the brain sections revealed that the neurons expressed the AAV-GCaMP6s virus in all three targeted brain areas but the number of GFP-expressing cells was different in each structure. The DG expressed an average of 50 ± 9 GFP cells distributed between the gyrus’s two blades, the dorsal blade expressed an average of 59 ± 11, and the ventral blade expressed an average of 41 ± 6 GFP labeled cells (Fig. 5A). Occasional (1 to 4 GFP cells) were also observed in the hippocampus’s pyramidal cell layer of field of the hippocampus (CA1). The neurons in various nuclei of the Amy were also labeled with GFP; 25 ± 4 GFP cells were expressed in the lateral nucleus (LA), 31 ± 5 GFP cells were counted in the basolateral nucleus (BLA) and 65 ± 8 GFP cells were found in the central nucleus (CeA). The total average number of GFP positive cells was 40 ± 6 for the entire Amy (Fig. 5B). The mPFC showed the lowest number of GFP-expressing cells found exclusively in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) subdivision of the mPFC with an average of 4 ± 2 GFP-labeled cells (Fig. 5C). These results support the hypothesis that mPFC activity can directly modulate the activity of LC neurons that project to limbic regions with a regulatory role in cognitive functions.

Fig. 4.

A diagram depicting the tripartite circuits that was revealed by anterograde transsynaptic cre and flip transfer. Injection of the anterograde tracer AAV1-hSyn-Cre (green, 1st order neuron) into the mPFC activated the cre-dependent anterograde tracer AAV9-pEF1a-DIO-FLPo (red, 2nd order neuron) injected into LC, which in turn, activated flip-dependent AAV8-pEf1a-fDIO-GCaMP6s (white, 3rd order neuron), injected respectively into the DG, Amy or mPFC in different groups of mice. DG, mPFC-LC-dentate gyrus; Amy, mPFC-LC-amygdala.

Fig. 5.

Third-order GFP labeled neurons were expressed by different limbic structures demonstrating continuous, tripartite circuits connecting mPFC to LC and LC to DG, Amy, and mPFC. (A) GFP-labeled cells occupy both blades of the DG, (B) lateral (LA), central (CeA), and basolateral (BLA) subnuclei of the Amy, and (C) the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Abbreviations: CA1, field of the hippocampus; PrL, prelimbic cortex; GFP, GCaMP6.

4. Discussion

Our results demonstrate direct involvement of the mPFC-LC neuronal projections in learning and memory processes. While optogenetic inhibition of the mPFC efferents in the pontine area of LC did not affect the general anxiety-like behavior, inhibition of the mPFC efferent fibers impaired object recognition memory, problem solving behavior, as well as CS-elicited freezing regardless of mPFC-LC inhibition during FC or testing. The collective behavioral data is supported by the results of our anterograde tracing experiment, which established continuous circuits from mPFC via LC to Amy, DG, and back to mPFC.

LC activity and NE release in the mPFC are pivotal in human cognitive control [34], and primate studies demonstrate an important role for the mPFC in tuning LC activity for optimal performance of a specific task [4]. This mPFC tuning of LC activity is critical, as the literature describes an inverted U-curve relationship between NE release and cognition, where either NE deficiency or excess hinders cognitive functions [4]. The different affinity of mPFC adrenergic receptors for NE is responsible for the dependence of cognitive performance on NE levels [35]. While too much NE release is associated with elevated anxiety levels, low NE levels lead to decreased arousal and attention. The results of our study align with the concept that the mPFC projections to LC are an essential regulatory element for optimal performance in a variety of cognitive tasks.

Pavlovian fear conditioning depends on an interconnected triad of brain structures composed of mPFC, DG, and Amy, where the LC activity and NE release into the triad is central for the acquisition, consolidation, and expression of aversive memories, for review see [36]. Both physical and psychological stressors activate LC with subsequent increased release of NE in Amy [17]. The auditory and nociceptive pathways that convey CS and US information converge onto the Amy and can trigger LTP and synaptogenesis, the basic mechanisms for associative learning, which are supported by optimal NE release [11, 14]. Interestingly, while most studies find that NE release into the Amy is critical for learning and memory processes, some studies find that NE is unnecessary for acquiring and consolidating aversive memories [37, 38]. The general explanation for this discrepancy is insufficient NE release, which strongly depends on stressor severity [39]. Only high-stress levels assure adequate activation of LC, which then delivers optimal NE in the Amy required for the acquisition of the fear association [39]. The selection of a single, high-intensity electric shock in our FC protocol is based the degree of fear expressed in response to the foot shock intensity [40, 41] equivalent to the degree of fear expressed to three weaker shocks [42]. We have shown that this protocol with a single but strong electric shock creates a longlisting fear response in mice [43]. Because LC activation depends on the strength of the foot shock [44], we believe that our single, strong US sufficiently activated LC to levels necessary for emotional augmentation of fear learning. This is supported by a reduction in freezing at test during CS presentations with inhibition of the efferents from the mPFC to LC. These results suggest a critical role for modulation of NE release by the LC via direct mPFC inputs in memory processes.

The initial fear engrams are created in the mPFC, Hipp and Amy but over time, only the engrams in the mPFC and Amy remain active upon recall [22, 45]. The storage of remote fear engrams in the mPFC may explain the results reported here, such that inhibition of the mPFC-LC projections may reduce the effect of fear engrams stored in the mPFC. This would therefore diminish the LC response to CS presentations and the accompanied expression of associative fear.

An important methodological shortcoming of our study is the need for direct evidence for changes in NE release in the targeted structures after inhibition of mPFC-LC neuronal projections. A detailed assessment of NE release in the limbic system after inhibition of the mPFC pontine efferents is our next step in this investigation. For now, the continuous tripartite neuronal circuits between the mPFC, LC, and Amy/DG/mPFC described here provide an anatomical pathway by which the mPFC may regulate NE release in the limbic system. The relatively small number of GFP-labeled neurons in observed in our experiment may be due to few synaptic contacts among targeted cells and NE efferent fibers, where NE released from free axonal endings exert effects via volume transmission [46, 47].

In summary, the study demonstrated the existence of continuous axonal pathways from the mPFC to the Amy, DG, and back to the mPFC via the LC. The mPFC-LC circuit is employed in distinct cognitive processes, such as learning, long-term memory, and problem-solving behavior, where mPFC regulation of LC activity is essential for optimal tuning of the NE release in the execution of memory tasks or solving a problem. In addition, this circuit is likely activated by intense stressors and participates in creating robust aversive memories by mPFC-mediated modulation of LC activity. This work may have far reaching implications for further investigation into the possible role of mPFC-mediated NE release in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), obsessive ruminations, memory loss, and other cognitive impairments.

5. Conclusions

In mice, the mPFC projections to LC are essential for optimal performance of cognitive tasks.

Availability of Data and Materials

All data points generated and/or analyzed during the current study are included in this article and there are no further underlying data necessary to reproduce the results.

Author Contributions

ED designed the study, performed the experiments, analyzed and reported the data. The author contributed to editorial changes in the manuscript. The author read and approved the final manuscript. The author had participated sufficiently in the work and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate

All animal work complies with the guidelines set by the IACUC at Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science, protocol number T23-10.

Acknowledgment

The author is very thankful to Andrea Cardenas for her technical assistance and Nicole Ferrara, PhD, for her editing help.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References
[1]
Jodo E, Chiang C, Aston-Jones G. Potent excitatory influence of prefrontal cortex activity on noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons. Neuroscience. 1998; 83: 63–79.
[2]
Sara SJ, Hervé-Minvielle A. Inhibitory influence of frontal cortex on locus coeruleus neurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1995; 92: 6032–6036.
[3]
Cardenas A, Papadogiannis A, Dimitrov E. The role of medial prefrontal cortex projections to locus ceruleus in mediating the sex differences in behavior in mice with inflammatory pain. FASEB Journal. 2021; 35: e21747.
[4]
Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. Adaptive gain and the role of the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system in optimal performance. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2005; 493: 99–110.
[5]
Chandler DJ, Jensen P, McCall JG, Pickering AE, Schwarz LA, Totah NK. Redefining Noradrenergic Neuromodulation of Behavior: Impacts of a Modular Locus Coeruleus Architecture. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019; 39: 8239–8249.
[6]
Poe GR, Foote S, Eschenko O, Johansen JP, Bouret S, Aston-Jones G, et al. Locus coeruleus: a new look at the blue spot. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2020; 21: 644–659.
[7]
Uematsu A, Tan BZ, Ycu EA, Cuevas JS, Koivumaa J, Junyent F, et al. Modular organization of the brainstem noradrenaline system coordinates opposing learning states. Nature Neuroscience. 2017; 20: 1602–1611.
[8]
Cahill L, McGaugh JL. Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends in Neurosciences. 1998; 21: 294–299.
[9]
Tovote P, Fadok JP, Lüthi A. Neuronal circuits for fear and anxiety. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2015; 16: 317–331.
[10]
Rogan MT, Stäubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature. 1997; 390: 604–607.
[11]
Herry C, Johansen JP. Encoding of fear learning and memory in distributed neuronal circuits. Nature Neuroscience. 2014; 17: 1644–1654.
[12]
Rumpel S, LeDoux J, Zador A, Malinow R. Postsynaptic receptor trafficking underlying a form of associative learning. Science. 2005; 308: 83–88.
[13]
Sapolsky RM. Stress and plasticity in the limbic system. Neurochemical Research. 2003; 28: 1735–1742.
[14]
Roozendaal B, Hui GK, Hui IR, Berlau DJ, McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM. Basolateral amygdala noradrenergic activity mediates corticosterone-induced enhancement of auditory fear conditioning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2006; 86: 249–255.
[15]
Quirarte GL, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Glucocorticoid enhancement of memory storage involves noradrenergic activation in the basolateral amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1997; 94: 14048–14053.
[16]
McReynolds JR, McIntyre CK. Emotional modulation of the synapse. Reviews in the Neurosciences. 2012; 23: 449–461.
[17]
Tanaka T, Yokoo H, Mizoguchi K, Yoshida M, Tsuda A, Tanaka M. Noradrenaline release in the rat amygdala is increased by stress: studies with intracerebral microdialysis. Brain Research. 1991; 544: 174–176.
[18]
Galvez R, Mesches MH, McGaugh JL. Norepinephrine release in the amygdala in response to footshock stimulation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 1996; 66: 253–257.
[19]
Tully K, Bolshakov VY. Emotional enhancement of memory: how norepinephrine enables synaptic plasticity. Molecular Brain. 2010; 3: 15.
[20]
van Stegeren AH. The role of the noradrenergic system in emotional memory. Acta Psychologica. 2008; 127: 532–541.
[21]
Seo DO, Zhang ET, Piantadosi SC, Marcus DJ, Motard LE, Kan BK, et al. A locus coeruleus to dentate gyrus noradrenergic circuit modulates aversive contextual processing. Neuron. 2021; 109: 2116–2130.e6.
[22]
Kitamura T, Ogawa SK, Roy DS, Okuyama T, Morrissey MD, Smith LM, et al. Engrams and circuits crucial for systems consolidation of a memory. Science. 2017; 356: 73–78.
[23]
Antunes M, Biala G. The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cognitive Processing. 2012; 13: 93–110.
[24]
Shepard R, Beckett E, Coutellier L. Assessment of the acquisition of executive function during the transition from adolescence to adulthood in male and female mice. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2017; 28: 29–40.
[25]
Ben Abdallah NMB, Fuss J, Trusel M, Galsworthy MJ, Bobsin K, Colacicco G, et al. The puzzle box as a simple and efficient behavioral test for exploring impairments of general cognition and executive functions in mouse models of schizophrenia. Experimental Neurology. 2011; 227: 42–52.
[26]
Galsworthy MJ, Paya-Cano JL, Liu L, Monleón S, Gregoryan G, Fernandes C, et al. Assessing reliability, heritability and general cognitive ability in a battery of cognitive tasks for laboratory mice. Behavior Genetics. 2005; 35: 675–692.
[27]
Han X, Chow BY, Zhou H, Klapoetke NC, Chuong A, Rajimehr R, et al. A high-light sensitivity optical neural silencer: development and application to optogenetic control of non-human primate cortex. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2011; 5: 18.
[28]
El-Gaby M, Zhang Y, Wolf K, Schwiening CJ, Paulsen O, Shipton OA. Archaerhodopsin Selectively and Reversibly Silences Synaptic Transmission through Altered pH. Cell Reports. 2016; 16: 2259–2268.
[29]
Setsuie R, Tamura K, Miyamoto K, Watanabe T, Takeda M, Miyashita Y. Off-Peak 594-nm Light Surpasses On-Peak 532-nm Light in Silencing Distant ArchT-Expressing Neurons In Vivo. iScience. 2020; 23: 101276.
[30]
Gabbott PLA, Warner TA, Jays PRL, Salway P, Busby SJ. Prefrontal cortex in the rat: projections to subcortical autonomic, motor, and limbic centers. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2005; 492: 145–177.
[31]
Allen Reference Atlas–Mouse Brain [brain atlas]. Available at: http://atlas.brain-map.org (Accessed: 15 November 2023).
[32]
Loughlin SE, Foote SL, Grzanna R. Efferent projections of nucleus locus coeruleus: morphologic subpopulations have different efferent targets. Neuroscience. 1986; 18: 307–319.
[33]
Zingg B, Chou XL, Zhang ZG, Mesik L, Liang F, Tao HW, et al. AAV-Mediated Anterograde Transsynaptic Tagging: Mapping Corticocollicular Input-Defined Neural Pathways for Defense Behaviors. Neuron. 2017; 93: 33–47.
[34]
Grueschow M, Kleim B, Ruff CC. Role of the locus coeruleus arousal system in cognitive control. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2020; 32: e12890.
[35]
Berridge CW, Spencer RC. Differential cognitive actions of norepinephrine a2 and a1 receptor signaling in the prefrontal cortex. Brain Research. 2016; 1641: 189–196.
[36]
Giustino TF, Maren S. Noradrenergic Modulation of Fear Conditioning and Extinction. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2018; 12: 43.
[37]
Ouyang M, Thomas SA. A requirement for memory retrieval during and after long-term extinction learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005; 102: 9347–9352.
[38]
Murchison CF, Zhang XY, Zhang WP, Ouyang M, Lee A, Thomas SA. A distinct role for norepinephrine in memory retrieval. Cell. 2004; 117: 131–143.
[39]
Díaz-Mataix L, Piper WT, Schiff HC, Roberts CH, Campese VD, Sears RM, et al. Characterization of the amplificatory effect of norepinephrine in the acquisition of Pavlovian threat associations. Learning & Memory. 2017; 24: 432–439.
[40]
Fanselow MS. Conditioned and unconditional components of post-shock freezing. The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science. 1980; 15: 177–182.
[41]
Cordero MI, Merino JJ, Sandi C. Correlational relationship between shock intensity and corticosterone secretion on the establishment and subsequent expression of contextual fear conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1998; 112: 885–891.
[42]
Kamprath K, Wotjak CT. Nonassociative learning processes determine expression and extinction of conditioned fear in mice. Learning & Memory. 2004; 11: 770–786.
[43]
Cardenas A, Blanca M, Dimitrov E. Persistent pain intensifies recall of consolidated fear memories. Neurobiology of Stress. 2019; 10: 100163.
[44]
Giustino TF, Ramanathan KR, Totty MS, Miles OW, Maren S. Locus Coeruleus Norepinephrine Drives Stress-Induced Increases in Basolateral Amygdala Firing and Impairs Extinction Learning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2020; 40: 907–916.
[45]
Fan X, Song J, Ma C, Lv Y, Wang F, Ma L, et al. Noradrenergic signaling mediates cortical early tagging and storage of remote memory. Nature Communications. 2022; 13: 7623.
[46]
Agnati LF, Zoli M, Strömberg I, Fuxe K. Intercellular communication in the brain: wiring versus volume transmission. Neuroscience. 1995; 69: 711–726.
[47]
Olschowka JA, Molliver ME, Grzanna R, Rice FL, Coyle JT. Ultrastructural demonstration of noradrenergic synapses in the rat central nervous system by dopamine-beta-hydroxylase immunocytochemistry. The Journal of Histochemistry and Cytochemistry. 1981; 29: 271–280.

Publisher’s Note: IMR Press stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share
Back to top