IMR Press / JIN / Volume 22 / Issue 2 / DOI: 10.31083/j.jin2202043
Open Access Original Research
Dynamic Changes of Behavioral Despair, HPA Axis Activity, and Hippocampal Neurogenesis in Male Rats Induced by Social Defeat Stress
Show Less
1 Department of Pharmacotherapeutics, Faculty of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Fukuoka University, 814-0180 Fukuoka, Japan
2 Division of Pharmacotherapeutics, Faculty of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Himeji Dokkyo University, 670-8524 Hyogo, Japan
3 Department of Drug Informatics, Faculty of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Fukuoka University, 814-0180 Fukuoka, Japan
*Correspondence: morimasa@fukuoka-u.ac.jp (Masayoshi Mori)
J. Integr. Neurosci. 2023, 22(2), 43; https://doi.org/10.31083/j.jin2202043
Submitted: 11 October 2022 | Revised: 28 November 2022 | Accepted: 2 December 2022 | Published: 20 February 2023
Copyright: © 2023 The Author(s). Published by IMR Press.
This is an open access article under the CC BY 4.0 license.
Abstract

Background: Psychosocial stress factors, such as threat and defeat, are major risk factors for the development of depression. The precise mechanisms underlying stress-induced depression are not clearly understood because the stress response in the brain varies in a stress-frequency-dependent manner. In the current research milieu on the pathogenesis of depression, the focus is on depression-like behavioral phenotype, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, and hippocampal neurogenesis. However, most studies have evaluated the symptomatic features of depression at certain time points after exposure to psychosocial stress. Here, we examined the frequency-dependent effects of psychosocial stress on depression-related features in rats. Methods: In the present study, different frequencies (one, two, three, or four times) of psychosocial stress were applied to 19 male Sprague-Dawley rats using a resident/intruder paradigm. Subsequently, the rats were subjected to a stress reactivity test to evaluate HPA axis activity, following which assessments of immobility behavior in the forced swimming test (FST) and adult neurogenesis were conducted. Results: One-time stressed rats showed a decrease in immobility behavior in the FST and the amount of doublecortin (DCX)-positive cells. Two-time stress caused hypoactivity of the HPA axis. In contrast, immobility behavior and HPA axis activity were increased after four-time stress exposure, but the number of DCX-positive cells was decreased. Conclusions: Our findings suggest that psychosocial stress produces a biphasic effect on the symptoms of depression in a stress-frequency-dependent manner, which could provide insights to facilitate further pathogenesis research on depression.

Keywords
depression
social defeat
stress frequency
hippocampal neurogenesis
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis
1. Introduction

Depression is one of the most prevalent psychiatric disorders, with a severe burden of disability worldwide [1, 2]. There is a large body of evidence indicating that stressors contribute to the development of depression [3, 4]. However, despite intensive research on the relationship between stress and depression, the exact molecular mechanism is largely unknown. The physiological response to stress in the brain varies depending on the nature and duration of stress [5, 6], making a clear understanding of the pathogenesis of depression very difficult.

Previous reports have demonstrated that acute stress causes adaptive physiological and behavioral responses to maintain homeostasis, while prolonged stress exposure causes maladaptive responses, such as depression [7, 8]. A previous study demonstrated that restraint stress induces opposite alterations of the glucocorticoid receptor (GR) and mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) expression levels in various brain regions depending on the exposure period [9]. These two receptors are associated with the regulation of habitual coping behaviors to stressful conditions, as well as hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity, and is known as one of the endocrinological mechanisms against stress stimuli [10, 11]. Furthermore, Huang et al. [12] found that chronic unpredictable mild stress (CUMS)-induced changes in depression-related factors varied at different stages of depression in an animal model during stress exposure. These observations suggest that the effects of stress on depression-related factors are different depending on the time point of assessment. Therefore, it is important to investigate the dynamic changes in the features of depression to further understanding of the pathogenesis of depression.

In addition to restraint stress and CUMS, social defeat stress is also considered to be useful for assessing depression because the most important factor for the development of depression in humans is a psychological stress event such as threat or defeat [13, 14, 15, 16, 17]. In preclinical studies, socially defeated animals exhibit various profound physiological and behavioral changes that are reminiscent of the pathologies observed in human patients with depression [18, 19]. Thus, the social defeat model is useful to better understand the pathogenesis of depression. However, to our knowledge, previous studies on the pathological changes in the social defeat model were mostly based on observations at single time points after stress exposure; changes at different points during stress exposure were not dynamically observed.

Adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus is a process by which new granule cell neurons are added throughout life to the dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus. Since the hippocampus is a brain region for regulating the emotion and stress response [20, 21], hippocampal neurogenesis dysfunction induced by stress exposure is an important hallmark for the development of depression [22, 23, 24]. Moreover, hippocampal neurogenesis plays a role in the regulation of the negative feedback of the HPA axis [25]. The HPA axis, when activated by stress, leads to corticotrophin-releasing hormone production in the hypothalamus that triggers adrenocorticotropic hormone release from the anterior pituitary and subsequent release of glucocorticoids (cortisol in humans and corticosterone [CORT] in rodents) from the adrenal cortex into circulation [10]. Neurogenesis impairment increases the stress response and blood glucocorticoid levels, and slows the recovery of HPA axis activity, which is associated with anxiety- and depression-like behaviors in rodents [25, 26]. Prolonged high levels of glucocorticoid leads to neuronal death or the failure of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in both humans and animal models of depression [27, 28, 29, 30, 31]. Moreover, numerous studies on social defeat stress have reported that stressed animals showed higher CORT levels, impairment of hippocampal neurogenesis, and increase in depression-like behaviors [32, 33, 34, 35, 36]. Based on the close relationship between hippocampal neurogenesis, HPA axis activity, and depression, it is necessary to dynamically observe these depression-related factors in social defeat models to better understand the pathogenesis of depression.

Our previous studies have demonstrated that an intermittent four-time exposure to social defeat stress using the resident/intruder paradigm caused impairments of hippocampal neurogenesis and affective behavior in male Sprague-Dawley rats [37, 38]. To reveal the dynamic changes in depression-related factors induced by social defeat stress in a stress-frequency-dependent manner, we exposed rats to social defeat stress one to four times. The different responses to these multiple social defeat stress exposures were evaluated in the socially defeated rats by examining depression-like behavior in the forced swimming test (FST), HPA axis activity response to acute stress in the stress reactivity test (SRT), and hippocampal neurogenesis.

2. Materials and Methods
2.1 Animals and Housing Conditions

A total of 23 male Sprague-Dawley rats (6 weeks of age at arrival; 160–190 g; Kyudo Co., Ltd., Saga, Japan) were used in this study. They were individually housed in plastic cages (23 × 14 × 12 cm3) under the following conditions: a 12:12 h light-dark cycle, light turned on at 7 AM, temperature of 22 ± 2 °C, and humidity of 55 ± 5%. They were able to access food and water ad libitum and were acclimated to the housing conditions for 1 week after arrival. To generate the paradigm of psychosocial stress, retired male Long-Evans rats (Kiwa Laboratory Animals Co., Ltd., Wakayama, Japan) were used as residents. In order to increase aggression and territorial behavior, these animals were pair-housed with females (6 weeks of age) in large plastic cages (50 × 40 × 20 cm3), as previously described [37]. We performed all animal care and used procedures under the regulations established by the Experimental Animal Care and Use Committee of Fukuoka University, which follow the universal principles of laboratory animal care (Aug. 24, 2015; approval number: 1508860).

2.2 Social Defeat Procedure Based on the Resident/Intruder Paradigm

The social defeat procedure in this study was adopted from a previous publication [39]. Briefly, each session of social defeat stress was conducted for 30 min. Before the start of the social defeat procedure, the resident female Long-Evans rat was removed, and the male was kept in the cage. A male Sprague-Dawley rat, an intruder, was introduced into the cage of a resident. Usually, the intruder rat is attacked by the aggressive resident male rats within 1–3 min and exhibits a submissive posture such as a supine posture and/or freezing behavior, which indicates that the intruder has been psychosocially defeated. Immediately after observing the defeat behaviors of the intruder, the intruder was moved into a protective wire mesh cage (15 × 20 × 15 cm3). This cage was placed in the center of the resident’s cage until the end of the session without direct physical contact with the resident. A different resident male rat was used for each session. Each session of social defeat stress was carried out between 10:30 AM. and 11:00 PM. Novel cage stress was applied to control rats to normalize any effects impaired by the novel environment. In short, the control rats were moved from their home cages to a new, larger cage in a new room with fresh bedding for 10 min, and then placed in a wire mesh cage for an additional 20 min.

2.3 Stress Reactivity Test and Plasma Corticosterone Levels

To test the reactivity of the HPA axis to acute stress, we performed the SRT according to a previous study with slight modifications [40]. The SRT is performed before and after the end of the social defeat stress period. All animals were restrained for 15 min in an acrylic rodent restrainer (KN-325-B, Natsume Seisakusho Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan). Immediately after the beginning (at 0 min) and at the termination (at 15 min) of restraint stress, blood samples were collected from the same rat via the tail vein using a 23-gauge needle (01045, Nipro, Osaka, Japan) and transferred into a tube containing 5 μL of heparin. Subsequently, the samples were centrifuged at 830 g for 15 min at 4 °C to separate the plasma, which was stored at –80 °C until assayed using a commercially available CORT ELISA kit according to the manufacturer’s instructions (EC3001-1, AssayPro, St. Charles, MO, USA). The delta (Δ) value of plasma CORT was calculated according to the formula: ΔCORT levels = plasma CORT levels at 15 min – plasma CORT levels at 0 min.

2.4 Experimental Design

All animals were acclimatized for 1 week after arrival and subjected to an SRT (pre-SRT). Three days after the pre-SRT, animals were randomly assigned to five experimental groups. (1) Control group animals (N = 4) were intermittently placed in a novel cage four times, once every 24–48 h, over the course of 8 days. (2) One-time-defeated group animals (N = 5) were intermittently placed in a novel cage three times and subsequently exposed to social defeat once, every 24–48 h, over the course of 8 days. (3) Two-time-defeated group animals (N = 5) were intermittently placed in a novel cage two times and subsequently exposed to social defeat twice, once every 24–48 h, over the course of 8 days. (4) Three-time-defeated group animals (N = 4) were intermittently placed in a novel cage and subsequently exposed to social defeat three times, once every 24–48 h, over the course of 8 days. (5) Four-time-defeated group animals (N = 5) were subjected to intermittent social defeat four times, once every 24–48 h, over the course of 8 days. One day after the last stress session, all animals were subjected to post-SRT. One day after post-SRT, an FST was performed. Two hours after the FST, the rats were transcardially perfused with fixative for the evaluation of hippocampal neurogenesis. Social defeat stress, FST, and SRT were performed between 9:30 AM and 1:30 PM The experimental design is illustrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Experimental design. All animals were subjected to a stress reactivity test (pre-SRT) 3 days before the start of the stress sessions. In the stress sessions, a total of four bouts of stress exposures (novel cage stress and/or social defeat stress) were conducted. (1) Control group (N = 4) animals were intermittently subjected to novel cage stress four times. (2) One-time-defeated group (N = 5) animals were intermittently subjected to novel cage stress three times and then to social defeat stress one time. (3) Two-time-defeated group (N = 5) animals were intermittently subjected to novel cage stress two times and then to social defeat stress two times. (4) Three-time-defeated group (N = 4) animals were intermittently subjected to novel cage stress one time and then to social defeat stress three times. (5) Four-time-defeated group (N = 5) animals were intermittently subjected to social defeat stress four times. In all groups, on the day after the last stress session, SRT (post-SRT) was conducted again. One day after the post-SRT, animals were subjected to the forced swim test (FST), then perfused transcardially with a fixative to evaluate hippocampal neurogenesis.

2.5 Forced Swimming Test (FST)

The FST was conducted as previously described [39]. Briefly, each rat was forced to swim in a Plexiglas cylinder, 20 cm in diameter and 50 cm in height, containing water filled to a depth of 30 cm at 27 ± 1 °C. Two swimming sessions were held. An initial 15-min “pre-test session” was carried out and subsequently, after 24 h, a 5-min “test session” was conducted. The test session was recorded by video and analyzed. Each rat was manually evaluated at 5-s intervals, and the predominant behavior was classified as: immobility, swimming, or climbing. After each test, the water was changed, and the cylinder was rinsed with clean water.

2.6 Immunohistochemistry

Two hours after the FST, sodium pentobarbital was used to deeply anesthetize the rats. Then, they were perfused transcardially with saline followed by 4% ice-cold paraformaldehyde in phosphate-buffered saline. The brain was resected from the skull and post-fixed in the same fixative at 4 °C overnight. A freezing microtome was used to cut 40-μm-thick sections coronally through the entire hippocampus (bregma –1.72 to –6.84 mm) [41]. Every eighth section throughout the DG, yielding a mean of 15 sections per brain, was analyzed using the avidin-biotin complex method for doublecortin (DCX) immunostaining as previously described [39]. In detail, these sections were incubated for 30 min in 3% H2O2 to eliminate endogenous peroxidases. We used 5% normal horse serum for blocking before incubating the sections overnight at 4 °C in goat anti-DCX (1:400, sc-8066, SantaCruz, Dallas, TX, USA). The next day, sections were rinsed and incubated for 2 h with secondary antibodies (1:200, BA-9500, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA), amplified with avidin-biotin complex (1:300, DAKO Japan, Kyoto, Japan), and visualized using diaminobenzidine (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA). The sections were air-dried and counterstained with hematoxylin, dehydrated, cleared in xylene, and cover-slipped.

2.7 Stereological Analysis of Doublecortin-Positive Cells in the Dentate Gyrus

The total number of DCX-positive cells was counted in all parts of the rostrocaudal extent of the DG in the granular cell layer using a 40× objective lens (Nikon E600, Nikon, Tokyo, Japan). The optical fractionator method was used for counting as described previously [37, 39].

2.8 Statistical Analyses

Data were analyzed using StatView software Ver. 5 (Hulinks, Tokyo, Japan). The data of each group were normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk test), except for the Δ plasma CORT levels of the four-time-defeated group in the pre-SRT, Δ plasma CORT levels of the one-time-defeated group in the post-SRT, and number of DCX-positive cells in the dorsal hippocampus. All data with a normal distribution exhibited homogeneity of variance (Levene’s test). One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare results of the FST and the number of DCX-positive cells in the ventral hippocampus of the five experimental groups. The Kruskal–Wallis test was performed for Δ plasma CORT levels in the pre- and post-SRT and the number of DCX-positive cells in the dorsal hippocampus, which were not normally distributed. Bonferroni/Dunn post-hoc analyses were performed to further examine between-group differences when ANOVA or Kruskal–Wallis tests demonstrated statistical significance. A paired t-test was used to compare the Δ plasma CORT levels between the pre- and post-SRT conditions in the control, two-, and three-time-defeated groups. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to compare the Δ plasma CORT levels between the pre- and post-SRT conditions in the one- and four-time-defeated groups. All data are represented as the mean ± standard error of the mean. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

3. Results
3.1 Effects of Various Frequencies of Social Defeat Stress on Behavioral Despair in the Forced Swimming Test

The effects of exposure to social defeat stress on the frequencies of immobility, climbing, and swimming behaviors in the FST are shown in Fig. 2. There were no significant changes in the frequency of swimming (Fig. 2, middle) and climbing (Fig. 2, right-hand side) behaviors between the groups. In contrast, one-way ANOVA indicated significant differences in the frequency of immobility behavior (F (4,18) = 8.239, p < 0.01; Fig. 2, left-hand side). Post-hoc analysis further showed a significantly lower immobility frequency in the one-time-defeated rats than in the two-time-defeated (p < 0.05), three-time-defeated (p < 0.01), and four-time-defeated rats (p < 0.0001). Furthermore, the one-time-defeated rats showed a significantly lower immobility frequency than the control rats (p < 0.05). The four-time-defeated rats displayed a significantly higher immobility frequency than the control rats (p < 0.05).

Fig. 2.

Effects of social defeat stress on animal behavior in the forced swimming test. One-time social defeat stress significantly decreased immobility compared with the control group in the forced swimming test, while four-time social defeat stress significantly increased immobility compared with the control group (left-hand side). Number of animals (N = 4–5) per group. Bars represent the mean ± standard error of the mean. Statistical analysis was performed by one-way analysis of variance followed by Bonferroni/Dunn post-hoc tests. ## p < 0.01, ### p < 0.0001 vs. one-time-defeated group.

3.2 Effects of Various Frequencies of Social Defeat Stress on The Increase in the Plasma Corticosterone Levels in the Stress Reaction Test

The effects of exposure to social defeat stress on the changes in plasma CORT levels in the SRT are shown in Fig. 3. We conducted the SRT both before the start (pre) and after the termination (post) of the social defeat stress procedure to examine the activity of the HPA axis response to acute restraint stress. At the pre-SRT, there was no significant difference in the increase (Δ) of plasma CORT levels between the groups (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Effects of social defeat stress on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity in the stress reactivity test. No effect of social defeat stress was observed in the stress reactivity test (pre-SRT, left-hand side). In contrast, social defeat stress significantly decreased Δ plasma corticosterone (CORT) levels in the two-time-defeated group and tended to increase Δ plasma CORT levels in the four-time-defeated group compared with the control group in the post-SRT (right-hand side). Number of animals (N = 4–5 per group). Bars represent the mean ± standard error of the mean. Statistical analysis was performed by Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Bonferroni/Dunn post-hoc tests. p < 0.01 vs. two-time-defeated group.

However, the Kruskal–Wallis test revealed a significant difference between the groups at the post-SRT (H = 11.266, p < 0.05; Fig. 3). Post-hoc analysis further showed higher Δ plasma CORT levels in the four-time-defeated rats than in the control rats (p = 0.0853); however, these differences were not significant. In contrast, the two-time-defeated rats displayed significantly lower Δ plasma CORT levels than the controls (p < 0.05), one-time-defeated (p < 0.01), three-time-defeated (p < 0.01), and four-time-defeated rats (p < 0.01). We analyzed the effects of exposure to social defeat stress on the difference in Δ plasma CORT levels between the pre- and post-SRT conditions within individual groups. In the two-time-defeated rats, Δ plasma CORT levels at pre- and post-SRT were 156.1 ± 10.5 and 93.6 ± 16.3 ng/mL, respectively, showing significant decrease (t (1,4)= 2.912, p < 0.05). In the four-time-defeated rats, Δ plasma CORT levels at pre- and post-SRT were 186.2 ± 32.2 and 250.7 ± 26.7 ng/mL, respectively, showing significant increase (Z = –2.023, p < 0.05). No statistically significant differences were observed in the other groups (Table 1).

Table 1.Differences in Δ plasma CORT levels between pre-SRT and post-SRT conditions.
Group Δ Plasma CORT levels (ng/mL) Pre- vs. post-SRT
Post-SRT – pre-SRT p value
Control 31.6 ± 26.6 0.3203
One-time-defeat 52.6 ± 34.3 0.1380
Two-time-defeat −62.5 ± 21.5 0.0436*
Three-time-defeat −12.3 ± 74.5 0.8792
Four-time-defeat 64.5 ± 11.0 0.0431*

CORT, corticosterone; SRT, stress reactivity test.

All values are expressed as the mean ± standard error of the mean. Comparisons were made between the Δ plasma CORT levels under pre- vs. post-SRT conditions within individual groups. Statistical analysis was performed using a paired t test for the control, two-, and three-time defeated groups and using the Wilcoxon signed rank test in the one- and four-time defeated groups.

*p < 0.05.

3.3 Effects of Various Frequencies of Social Defeat Stress on Doublecortin-Positive Cells in the Dentate Gyrus of the Dorsal and Ventral Hippocampus

The number of DCX-positive cells in the DG resulting from multiple social defeat stress is shown in Fig. 4. DCX-labeled cells were observed mainly in the subgranular zone, which borders the hippocampal DG (Fig. 4a). Because of technical constraints, brain samples for immunohistochemistry could not be obtained from one animal in the two-time-defeated group. A separate analysis was conducted on the dorsal and ventral parts of the DG, with the number of DCX-positive cells in the subgranular zone expressed per unit volume of the corresponding DG. No statistically significant difference was observed in the density of DCX-positive cells in the dorsal DG of the hippocampus (Fig. 4b, left-hand side). Contrastingly, one-way ANOVA indicated a significant difference in the number of DCX-positive cells in the ventral hippocampus (F (4,17) = 3.077, p < 0.05; right-hand side). Compared with the control rats, post-hoc analysis further revealed a significantly lower number of DCX-positive cells in the one-time- (p < 0.05), three-time- (p < 0.05), and four-time-defeated rats (p < 0.01).

Fig. 4.

Photomicrographs of representative doublecortin (DCX)-immunopositive cells in the dorsal and ventral hippocampal dentate gyrus of the rats in the five groups (a; magnification: × 40; scale bars: 100 μm, × 400; scale bars: 10 μm). Effects of social defeat stress on DCX-positive cells (a). Numbers of DCX-labeled cells in the subgranular zone in the dentate gyrus per volume of granular cell layer in the dorsal (left-hand side) and ventral hippocampus (right-hand side). In the dorsal hippocampus, there was no significant difference in the density of DCX-positive cells between the groups (b). In the ventral hippocampus, social defeat stress significantly decreased the density of DCX-positive cells in the one-, three-, and four-time-defeated groups compared with the control group. Bars represent the mean ± standard error of the mean. Statistical analysis was performed by the Kruskal–Wallis test for the number of DCX-positive cells in the dorsal hippocampus and by one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni/Dunn post-hoc tests for the number of DCX-positive cells in the ventral hippocampus.

4. Discussion

In the current study, we assessed immobility behavior in the FST, activity of the HPA axis response to acute restraint stress, and the number of immature neurons in the DG of the hippocampus in rats subjected to different frequencies of social defeat stress exposure. We found that one or two stress exposures caused a decrease in immobility behavior, HPA axis activity, and the number of immature neurons. In contrast, immobility behavior and HPA axis activity of stressed rats increased with four stress exposures, but the number of immature neurons was decreased. These results suggest that social defeat stress produces dynamic changes in depression-related factors in a stress frequency-dependent manner.

We also found that social defeat stress using the resident-intruder paradigm induced opposite alterations in immobility behavior in the one-time- vs. four-time-defeated groups. One-time-stress exposure significantly decreased immobility, while four-time-defeated rats exhibited significantly increased immobility behavior. The FST is one of the most commonly used tests to assess behavioral despair or depression-like state in rodents [42, 43]. Chronic exposure to social defeat stress increases the duration of immobility, which can be decreased by antidepressant treatment [17, 44, 45, 46]. Thus, our results suggest that one-time social defeat stress exposure causes antidepressant-like effects, while four-time-stress exposure leads to a depressive state. In particular, the positive (shortening) effect of acute social defeat stress on immobility behavior in the FST is a particularly important result while considering that psychosocial stress is a common risk factor for the development of depression.

Regarding the depressive phenotype induced, we confirmed that rats who underwent four-time-social defeat stress showed slightly increased HPA axis activity in the post-SRT (p = 0.0853) and significantly increased Δ plasma CORT levels between the pre- and post-SRT conditions. Furthermore, four-time-social defeat stress was associated with significantly decreased DCX-positive cells in the DG of the ventral hippocampus. The hippocampus is functionally segregated along the dorsolateral/ventromedial axis. The dorsal (septal) region is involved in numerous cognitive functions, while the ventral (temporal) region is linked to emotional behaviors and negative feedback regulation of HPA axis activation induced by stress [20, 21, 25]. Thus, the impairment of ventral hippocampal neurogenesis is considered more important for the development of depressive symptoms, such as depression-like behavior and HPA axis hyperactivity [47, 48]. DCX is a microtubule-associated protein that serves as a surrogate marker of adult neurogenesis due to its transient expression in newborn neurons [49, 50]. Previous studies have demonstrated that social defeat stress reduced hippocampal neurogenesis [32, 33, 37, 38, 51]. Additionally, repeated social defeat stress induced higher plasma CORT levels, indicative of hyperactivity of the HPA axis [36, 52]. Thus, these results suggest that the four-time social defeat stress paradigm induced a typical depression-like phenotype with behavioral despair, HPA axis hyperactivity, and impairment of hippocampal neurogenesis.

As mentioned above, hippocampal neurogenesis has been implicated in the regulation of depression-like behavior and the normalization of HPA axis activity after stress [25, 47, 53]; however, one-time stressed rats did not show an increase in depression-like behavior and HPA axis hyperactivity despite a decrease in neuroblasts/immature neurons in the hippocampus. One possible explanation for this discrepancy is that the impact of stress-induced increases in CORT levels on hippocampal functions may differ in a time-dependent manner.

Glucocorticoid receptors (GR and MR) are highly expressed in the hippocampus. Moreover, as neural stem cells in the DG of the hippocampus are located in close vicinity to blood vessels [54], stress and stress-induced elevated CORT levels may affect hippocampal functions and neurogenesis in the DG [30, 55]. Acute stress-induced increase in CORT has a positive effect on hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation, whereas chronic stress and prolonged exposure to elevated CORT levels have been associated with deficits in learning, memory, and retrieval [56, 57, 58, 59]. These findings support the possibility that acute and chronic social defeat stress may produce positive and negative effects on hippocampal functions, including negative feedback regulation of the HPA axis, respectively.

Regarding the positive effect of stress exposure on hippocampal neurogenesis, acute immobilization stress increases cell proliferation in the DG of the hippocampus [60]. In addition, acute glucocorticoid treatment enhanced phosphorylation of tropomyosin receptor kinase B and extracellular signal regulated kinase 1/2 in hippocampal neurons [61, 62], which are key mediators of increasing neurogenesis and the therapeutic response to antidepressants [63, 64]. Several studies have demonstrated that fluoxetine, an antidepressant, accelerates the maturation of immature neurons by shortening the immature stage of newborn cells, such as the DCX-expressing time window in the hippocampus [65, 66]. Thus, it can be speculated that one-time (acute) social defeat stress increases the maturation of newborn neurons, presumably shortening the DCX-positive stage and thereby lowering the number of cells at this stage at the time of analysis. The enhanced and accelerated hippocampal neurogenesis, induced by acute social defeat stress, may induce antidepressant-like behavior (decreased immobility in the FST) and normalization of HPA axis activity (a hippocampal function), whereas prolonged stress exposure, such as the four-time-defeated group in this study, may cause depressive behavior and HPA axis hyperactivity by decreasing hippocampal neurogenesis.

Notably, we observed interesting results in the two-time- and three-time-defeated groups. First, the two-time-defeated rats had a significant decrease in HPA axis activity compared with the other groups under the post-SRT condition (Fig. 3, right-hand side). Experiencing social defeat stress twice decreased the Δ plasma CORT levels between the pre- and post-SRT conditions in this group, indicating hypoactivity of the HPA axis. Nevertheless, immobility behavior in the FST and hippocampal neurogenesis was unchanged in this group.

Since this is a novel aspect of depression-related change induced by social defeat stress that has never been studied, the precise mechanisms are not known. However, a previous study showed that glucocorticoid pretreatment diminished the subsequent restraint stress-induced adrenocorticotropic hormone and CORT release in rats by GR occupancy in the hippocampus, the primary negative feedback mediator of the HPA axis [67]. Furthermore, several studies on social defeat stress have reported that plasma CORT levels were increased immediately after stress exposure, and repeated social defeat stress induced higher plasma CORT levels for a prolonged period after the last stress session [34, 36, 52, 68]. These observations suggest that two bouts (repeated) of social defeat stress elevated plasma CORT levels for a long duration in the hippocampus, similar to a CORT pretreatment condition that may suppress the activity of the HPA axis response to restraint stress in the two-time-defeated group. Subsequently, hypoactivity of the HPA axis may offset the positive effects of CORT on hippocampal neurogenesis and immobility behavior that appeared in the one-time-defeated rats.

Next, the three-time-defeated rats had a decreased number of immature neurons in the hippocampus, but immobility and activity of the HPA axis were comparable to those of the control rats in this study. We speculate that the reduction of immature neurons resulted from the increase in stress exposure frequency because four bouts of social defeat stress led to a depression phenotype. Hill et al. [69] showed that a certain level of neurogenesis is required for exerting its functions. Based on this, it is possible that three bouts of stress led to a prodromal phase of depression during which the decrease in adult-generated neurons that functionally integrate into hippocampal circuits is not at a level that causes functional deficits. To obtain precise data on hippocampal neurogenesis to further support our hypothesis, future studies should assess the ratio of DCX-positive cells to Ki-67-positive cells, which is useful as a proxy for cell maturation speed and survival [70], and use other maturation markers, such as NeuN, PSD95, and calbindin, and the dendritic complexity of DCX-positive cells should be quantified. Additionally, GR and MR are densely expressed by granule cell neurons in the DG of the hippocampus and play a crucial role in HPA axis regulation and depression [71, 72, 73]. A previous study demonstrated that restraint stress dynamically changed GR and MR expression levels in various brain regions, including the hippocampus, depending on the exposure period [9]. For instance, acute restraint stress induced an increase in hippocampal MR protein levels and decrease in hippocampal GR protein levels. Conversely, repeated restraint stress downregulated hippocampal MR protein levels [9]. Therefore, we plan to examine the effects of social defeat stress on hippocampal GR and MR levels at varying frequencies of stress exposure in the future.

A recent study demonstrated that a CUMS-induced animal model of depression exhibited dynamic changes in depression-related features at different stages after the onset of depression [12]. Thus, to our knowledge, the present study is the first to show the dynamic changes in depression-related features in a social defeat animal model at the preclinical stage of depression (the stage from the start of stress exposure to the development of depression).

However, our study has several limitations. First, considering that the number of rats in each group was low (N = 4–5), caution should be exercised when interpreting the results or drawing conclusions. Therefore, further statistical analyses in a future study with sufficient sample sizes are warranted. Second, we used only the FST to assess depression-like behaviors. Thus, future studies on the dynamic effects of social defeat stress on other depression-related behaviors should use additional tests, such as the sucrose preference test and tail suspension test. Third, we used only male rats, which are highly motivated to defend their territory against unfamiliar males. The resident/intruder paradigm cannot be used with female rodents since they do not demonstrate territorial behaviors [14]. However, in humans, women are two to three times more likely to develop depression than men [74]. Therefore, future studies should assess stress-induced dynamic changes in depression-related factors in a stress frequency-dependent or duration-dependent manner in female rats using a different stress paradigm, such as CUMS.

5. Conclusions

In summary, our data showed that social defeat stress, using the resident/intruder paradigm, could produce a biphasic effect on symptomatic features of depression, such as behavioral despair, HPA axis activity, and hippocampal neurogenesis, in a stress-frequency-dependent manner. Acute social defeat stress (one-time-defeat) enhances a coping response to stressful conditions, whereas repeated stress exposure (four-time-defeat) leads to continuous maladaptive stress responses, possibly breaking down to a chronic psychopathological disease state of depression.

Availability of Data and Materials

The datasets used and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author Contributions

HH, MM, YM, KO, and ME designed the study. HH, MM, and SK acquired the data, and HH, MM, YM, KT, and TM analyzed the data. HH and MM wrote the article, which all authors reviewed and approved for publication.

Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate

We performed all animal care and use procedures under the regulations established by the Experimental Animal Care and Use Committee of Fukuoka University, which follow universal principles of laboratory animal care (Aug. 24, 2015; approval number: 1508860).

Acknowledgment

We would like to thank Editage (https://www.editage.com/) for English language editing.

Funding

This work was supported in part by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI Grant Number 20K16657.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References
[1]
Cameron C, Habert J, Anand L, Furtado M. Optimizing the management of depression: primary care experience. Psychiatry Research. 2014; Suppl 1: S45–S57.
[2]
Gunnar M, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review Psychology. 2007; 58: 145–173.
[3]
Krishnan V, Nestler EJ. The molecular neurobiology of depression. Nature. 2008; 455: 894–902.
[4]
Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brüne M, Bullmore ET, Carter CS, Clayton NS, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews. Drug Discovery. 2012; 11: 141–168.
[5]
Papadopoulou A, Siamatras T, Delgado-Morales R, Amin ND, Shukla V, Zheng YL, et al. Acute and chronic stress differentially regulate cyclin-dependent kinase 5 in mouse brain: implications to glucocorticoid actions and major depression. Translational Psychiatry. 2015; 5: e578.
[6]
Saaltink DJ, Vreugdenhil E. Stress, glucocorticoid receptors, and adult neurogenesis: a balance between excitation and inhibition? Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences. 2014; 71: 2499–2515.
[7]
Charney DS. Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience and vulnerability: implications for successful adaptation to extreme stress. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004; 161: 195–216.
[8]
McEwen BS. Sex, stress and the hippocampus: allostasis, allostatic load and the aging process. Neurobiology of Aging. 2002; 23: 921–939.
[9]
Gądek-Michalska A, Spyrka J, Rachwalska P, Tadeusz J, Bugajski J. Influence of chronic stress on brain corticosteroid receptors and HPA axis activity. Pharmacological Reports. 2013; 65: 1163–1175.
[10]
de Kloet ER, Oitzl MS, Joëls M. Stress and cognition: are corticosteroids good or bad guys? Trends in Neurosciences. 1999; 22: 422–426.
[11]
Schwabe L, Schächinger H, de Kloet ER, Oitzl MS. Corticosteroids operate as a switch between memory systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010; 22: 1362–1372.
[12]
Huang YL, Zeng NX, Chen J, Niu J, Luo WL, Liu P, et al. Dynamic changes of behaviors, dentate gyrus neurogenesis and hippocampal miR-124 expression in rats with depression induced by chronic unpredictable mild stress. Neural Regeneration Research. 2020; 15: 1150–1159.
[13]
Brown GW. Genetic and population perspectives on life events and depression. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 1998; 33: 363–372.
[14]
Björkqvist K. Social defeat as a stressor in humans. Physiology & Behavior. 2001; 73: 435–442.
[15]
Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Prescott CA. Causal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999; 156: 837–841.
[16]
Kessler RC. The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annual Review of Psychology. 1997; 48: 191–214.
[17]
Pine DS, Cohen P, Johnson JG, Brook JS. Adolescent life events as predictors of adult depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2002; 68: 49–57.
[18]
Hollis F, Kabbaj M. Social defeat as an animal model for depression. ILAR Journal. 2014; 55: 221–232.
[19]
Patel D, Kas MJ, Chattarji S, Buwalda B. Rodent models of social stress and neuronal plasticity: relevance to depressive-like disorders. Behavioural Brain Research. 2019; 369: 111900.
[20]
Moser MB, Moser EI. Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 1998; 8: 608–619.
[21]
Sahay A, Hen R. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression. Nature Neuroscience. 2007; 10: 1110–1115.
[22]
Dean J, Keshavan M. The neurobiology of depression: An integrated view. Asian Journal of Psychiatry. 2017; 27: 101–111.
[23]
Kempermann G, Kronenberg G. Depressed new neurons–adult hippocampal neurogenesis and a cellular plasticity hypothesis of major depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2003; 54: 499–503.
[24]
Park SC. Neurogenesis and antidepressant action. Cell and Tissue Research. 2019; 377: 95–106.
[25]
Snyder JS, Soumier A, Brewer M, Pickel J, Cameron HA. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis buffers stress responses and depressive behaviour. Nature. 2011; 476: 458–461.
[26]
Schloesser RJ, Manji HK, Martinowich K. Suppression of adult neurogenesis leads to an increased hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis response. Neuroreport. 2009; 20: 553–557.
[27]
Cameron HA, Gould E. Adult neurogenesis is regulated by adrenal steroids in the dentate gyrus. Neuroscience. 1994; 61: 203–209.
[28]
Pariante CM. Risk factors for development of depression and psychosis. Glucocorticoid receptors and pituitary implications for treatment with antidepressant and glucocorticoids. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2009; 1179: 144–152.
[29]
Mirescu C, Gould E. Stress and adult neurogenesis. Hippocampus. 2006; 16: 233–238.
[30]
Tae WS, Kim SS, Lee KU, Nam EC, Choi JW, Park JI. Hippocampal shape deformation in female patients with unremitting major depressive disorder. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2011; 32: 671–676.
[31]
Tanapat P, Hastings NB, Rydel TA, Galea LA, Gould E. Exposure to fox odor inhibits cell proliferation in the hippocampus of adult rats via an adrenal hormone-dependent mechanism. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 2001; 437: 496–504.
[32]
Czéh B, Michaelis T, Watanabe T, Frahm J, de Biurrun G, van Kampen M, et al. Stress-induced changes in cerebral metabolites, hippocampal volume, and cell proliferation are prevented by antidepressant treatment with tianeptine. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001; 98: 12796–12801.
[33]
Czéh B, Welt T, Fischer AK, Erhardt A, Schmitt W, Müller MB, et al. Chronic psychosocial stress and concomitant repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: effects on stress hormone levels and adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Biological Psychiatry. 2002; 52: 1057–1065.
[34]
Kinsey SG, Bailey MT, Sheridan JF, Padgett DA, Avitsur R. Repeated social defeat causes increased anxiety-like behavior and alters splenocyte function in C57BL/6 and CD-1 mice. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2007; 21: 458–466.
[35]
Trainor BC, Pride MC, Villalon Landeros R, Knoblauch NW, Takahashi EY, Silva AL, et al. Sex differences in social interaction behavior following social defeat stress in the monogamous California mouse (Peromyscus californicus). PLoS ONE. 2011; 6: e17405.
[36]
Becker C, Zeau B, Rivat C, Blugeot A, Hamon M, Benoliel JJ. Repeated social defeat-induced depression-like behavioral and biological alterations in rats: involvement of cholecystokinin. Molecular Psychiatry. 2008; 13: 1079–1092.
[37]
Murata Y, Yanagihara Y, Mori M, Mine K, Enjoji M. Chronic treatment with tandospirone, a serotonin 1A receptor partial agonist, inhibits psychosocial stress-induced changes in hippocampal neurogenesis and behavior. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2015; 180: 1–9.
[38]
Murata Y, Matsuda H, Mikami Y, Hirose S, Mori M, Ohe K, et al. Chronic administration of quetiapine stimulates dorsal hippocampal proliferation and immature neurons of male rats, but does not reverse psychosocial stress-induced hyponeophagic behavior. Psychiatry Research. 2019; 272: 411–418.
[39]
Mori M, Murata Y, Tsuchihashi M, Hanakita N, Terasaki F, Harada H, et al. Continuous psychosocial stress stimulates BMP signaling in dorsal hippocampus concomitant with anxiety-like behavior associated with differential modulation of cell proliferation and neurogenesis. Behavioural Brain Research. 2020; 392: 112711.
[40]
Touma C, Bunck M, Glasl L, Nussbaumer M, Palme R, Stein H, et al. Mice selected for high versus low stress reactivity: a new animal model for affective disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008; 33: 839–862.
[41]
Paxinos G, Watson C, The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. sixth ed. Academic Press: New York. 2007.
[42]
Cryan JF, Valentino RJ, Lucki I. Assessing substrates underlying the behavioral effects of antidepressants using the modified rat forced swimming test. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2005; 29: 547–569.
[43]
Porsolt RD, Le Pichon M, Jalfre M. Depression: a new animal model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Nature. 1977; 266: 730–732.
[44]
Rygula R, Abumaria N, Flügge G, Fuchs E, Rüther E, Havemann-Reinecke U. Anhedonia and motivational deficits in rats: impact of chronic social stress. Behavioural Brain Research. 2005; 162: 127–134.
[45]
Rygula R, Abumaria N, Domenici E, Hiemke C, Fuchs E. Effects of fluoxetine on behavioral deficits evoked by chronic social stress in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 2006; 174: 188–192.
[46]
Rygula R, Abumaria N, Havemann-Reinecke U, Rüther E, Hiemke C, Zernig G, et al. Pharmacological validation of a chronic social stress model of depression in rats: effects of reboxetine, haloperidol and diazepam. Behavioural Pharmacology. 2008; 19: 183–196.
[47]
Jacobs BL, van Praag H, Gage FH. Adult brain neurogenesis and psychiatry: a novel theory of depression. Molecular Psychiatry. 2000; 5: 262–269.
[48]
Tanti A, Belzung C. Neurogenesis along the septo-temporal axis of the hippocampus: are depression and the action of antidepressants region-specific? Neuroscience. 2013; 252: 234–252.
[49]
Couillard-Despres S, Winner B, Schaubeck S, Aigner R, Vroemen M, Weidner N, et al. Doublecortin expression levels in adult brain reflect neurogenesis. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005; 21: 1–14.
[50]
Rao MS, Shetty AK. Efficacy of doublecortin as a marker to analyse the absolute number and dendritic growth of newly generated neurons in the adult dentate gyrus. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2004; 19: 234–246.
[51]
Lagace DC, Donovan MH, DeCarolis NA, Farnbauch LA, Malhotra S, Berton O, et al. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis is functionally important for stress-induced social avoidance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010; 107: 4436–4441.
[52]
Ruis MA, te Brake JH, Buwalda B, De Boer SF, Meerlo P, Korte SM, et al. Housing familiar male wildtype rats together reduces the long-term adverse behavioural and physiological effects of social defeat. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999; 24: 285–300.
[53]
Anacker C, Luna VM, Stevens GS, Millette A, Shores R, Jimenez JC, et al. Hippocampal neurogenesis confers stress resilience by inhibiting the ventral dentate gyrus. Nature. 2018; 559: 98–102.
[54]
Wurmser AE, Palmer TD, Gage FH. Neuroscience. Cellular interactions in the stem cell niche. Science. 2004; 304: 1253–1255.
[55]
Swaab DF, Bao AM, Lucassen PJ. The stress system in the human brain in depression and neurodegeneration. Ageing Research Reviews. 2005; 4: 141–194.
[56]
Bodnoff SR, Humphreys AG, Lehman JC, Diamond DM, Rose GM, Meaney MJ. Enduring effects of chronic corticosterone treatment on spatial learning, synaptic plasticity, and hippocampal neuropathology in young and mid-aged rats. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1995; 15: 61–69.
[57]
Dachir S, Kadar T, Robinzon B, Levy A. Cognitive deficits induced in young rats by long-term corticosterone administration. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1993; 60: 103–109.
[58]
McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiological Reviews. 2007; 87: 873–904.
[59]
Sandi C, Rose SP. Protein synthesis- and fucosylation-dependent mechanisms in corticosterone facilitation of long-term memory in the chick. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1997; 111: 1098–1104.
[60]
Kirby ED, Muroy SE, Sun WG, Covarrubias D, Leong MJ, Barchas LA, et al. Acute stress enhances adult rat hippocampal neurogenesis and activation of newborn neurons via secreted astrocytic FGF2. Elife. 2013; 2: e00362.
[61]
Jafari M, Seese RR, Babayan AH, Gall CM, Lauterborn JC. Glucocorticoid receptors are localized to dendritic spines and influence local actin signaling. Molecular Neurobiology. 2012; 46: 304–315.
[62]
Jeanneteau F, Garabedian MJ, Chao MV. Activation of Trk neurotrophin receptors by glucocorticoids provides a neuroprotective effect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008; 105: 4862–4867.
[63]
D’Sa C, Duman RS. Antidepressants and neuroplasticity. Bipolar Disorders. 2002; 4: 183–194.
[64]
Numakawa T, Adachi N, Richards M, Chiba S, Kunugi H. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and glucocorticoids: reciprocal influence on the central nervous system. Neuroscience. 2013; 239: 157–172.
[65]
Klempin F, Babu H, De Pietri Tonelli D, Alarcon E, Fabel K, Kempermann G. Oppositional effects of serotonin receptors 5-HT1a, 2, and 2c in the regulation of adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 2010; 3: 14.
[66]
Wang JW, David DJ, Monckton JE, Battaglia F, Hen R. Chronic fluoxetine stimulates maturation and synaptic plasticity of adult-born hippocampal granule cells. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2008; 28: 1374–1384.
[67]
Ginsberg AB, Campeau S, Day HE, Spencer RL. Acute glucocorticoid pretreatment suppresses stress-induced hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis hormone secretion and expression of corticotropin-releasing hormone hnRNA but does not affect c-fos mRNA or fos protein expression in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2003; 15: 1075–1083.
[68]
Watt MJ, Burke AR, Renner KJ, Forster GL. Adolescent male rats exposed to social defeat exhibit altered anxiety behavior and limbic monoamines as adults. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009; 123: 564–576.
[69]
Hill AS, Sahay A, Hen R. Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to reduce anxiety and depression-like behaviors. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015; 40: 2368–2378.
[70]
Wiget F, van Dijk RM, Louet ER, Slomianka L, Amrein I. Effects of strain and species on the septo-temporal distribution of adult neurogenesis in rodents. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2017; 11: 719.
[71]
de Kloet ER, Karst H, Joëls M. Corticosteroid hormones in the central stress response: quick-and-slow. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2008; 29: 268–272.
[72]
Levy BH, Tasker JG. Synaptic regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and its modulation by glucocorticoids and stress. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience. 2012; 6: 24.
[73]
McEwen BS. Stress and hippocampal plasticity. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1999; 22: 105–122.
[74]
Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Swartz M, Blazer DG, Nelson CB. Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. I: lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1993; 29: 85–96.

Publisher’s Note: IMR Press stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share
Back to top