IMR Press / JIN / Volume 21 / Issue 2 / DOI: 10.31083/j.jin2102067
Open Access Review
Hand movements in Mild Cognitive Impairment: clinical implications and insights for future research
Show Less
1 Department of Psychology, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, 81100 Caserta, Italy
2 Neurological Unit, CTO Hospital, AORN “Ospedali Dei Colli”, 80131 Naples, Italy
3 Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, 80138 Naples, Italy
4 Laboratory of Cognitive Science and Immersive Virtual Reality, Department of Psychology, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, 81100 Caserta, Italy
*Correspondence: cirorosario.ilardi@unicampania.it (Ciro Rosario Ilardi)
These authors contributed equally.
Academic Editor: Rafael Franco
J. Integr. Neurosci. 2022, 21(2), 67; https://doi.org/10.31083/j.jin2102067
Submitted: 4 November 2021 | Revised: 6 January 2022 | Accepted: 8 January 2022 | Published: 23 March 2022
Copyright: © 2022 The Author(s). Published by IMR Press.
This is an open access article under the CC BY 4.0 license.
Abstract

Decreased upper-extremity/visuomotor abilities are frequently encountered in healthy aging. However, few studies have assessed hand movements in the prodromal stage of dementia. The evaluation of visuomotor skills in patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment (PwMCI) may have non-negligible clinical relevance both in diagnostic and prognostic terms, given the strong relationships with executive functioning and functional autonomies. In the present review paper, these issues will be disclosed by describing general pathophysiological and neuropsychological mechanisms responsible for visuomotor deficits, and by reporting the available experimental results on differences in visuomotor functioning between PwMCI, healthy controls and/or patients with dementia. Moreover, the relationships binding visuomotor and executive domains to functional autonomies will be then addressed. Finally, we will propose insights for future research.

Keywords
Mild Cognitive Impairment
Dementia
Alzheimer's disease
Aging
Visuomotor
Hand movements
Movement planning
Motor control
Executive functions
ADL
1. Introduction

According to the World Health Organization (WHO), almost 10 million people are diagnosed with dementia each year. Nowadays, dementia affects about 50 million people worldwide. This figure is expected to increase to 82 million in 2030 and 152 million by 2050 [1]. Predictably, dementia imposes an extreme economic cost that is globally estimated to be around 818 billion dollars per year; therefore, it represents a significant barrier to both social and economic development [1, 2]. Hence there is a need for special attention on prevention and management of dementia risk factors including age-related psychophysiological decline, unhealthy lifestyle habits [3, 4], environmental factors [5], cardiocerebrovascular diseases [6, 7], sleep disturbances [8, 9], anxiety [10, 11], and apathetic symptoms [12, 13], particularly in patients suffering from Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) [14, 15, 16, 17, 18].

MCI is widely considered a transitional or boundary stage between normal aging and dementia [14]. MCI prevalence increases with age, from 6.7% in individuals aged 60–64 years to 25.2% in individuals aged 80–84 years [18]. Similarly, the estimates of MCI incidence range from 22.5% in individuals aged 75–79 years to 60.1% in individuals aged 85 years and older [19]. Overall, the cumulative incidence of conversion towards dementia is about 15% in patients with MCI (PwMCI) aged 65 years and over [17, 18]. Some PwMCI may remain stable or reverse to normal cognition; however, these individuals are still at a higher risk of progression back to MCI or conversion to dementia than individuals who have never received a diagnosis of MCI [18].

Based on the clinical algorithm proposed by Petersen [14, 15, 16], the diagnosis of MCI should be carried out in the presence of subjective cognitive complaints—referred by the patient and confirmed by an informant (e.g., a relative or the General Practitioner)—associated with cognitive deficit(s) formally detected by neuropsychological testing. Furthermore, PwMCI should not exhibit impairment in functional autonomies (i.e., activities of daily living, ADL) nor signs of overt dementia. According to the cognitive profile, four syndromic phenotypes have been outlined: the amnestic MCI–single domain (aMCI) variant, the amnestic MCI–multiple domain (aMCI-md) variant, the non-amnestic MCI–single domain (naMCI) variant, and the non-amnestic MCI–multiple domain (naMCI-md) variant. The diagnosis of aMCI is postulated in the presence of a selective memory deficit. Whether memory deficits are accompanied by an impairment in at least one other cognitive domain (e.g., language, executive functions, visuospatial skills), the diagnosis of aMCI-md is instead justified. Diagnosis of naMCI is advanced when a single non-memory domain is impaired, whereas naMCI-md refers to a clinical picture characterized by impairment in multiple non-memory domains [16].

Petersen’s diagnostic criteria, however, have some limits. For instance, several studies have demonstrated that PwMCI can show poor or no awareness of their cognitive deficits (i.e., anosognosia) and therefore no cognitive complaints [20, 21, 22]. Moreover, awareness of one’s deficits is, per se, a neuropsychological domain of clinical interest; thus, PwMCI showing both memory impairments and anosognosia for memory deficits should be reclassified from aMCI to aMCI-md. In addition, some evidence has suggested that MCI is often accompanied by functional decline in both basic (BADL, e.g., feeding, dressing, bathing, toileting, grooming) [23] and instrumental activities of daily living (IADL, e.g., managing finances and medications, shopping, cooking, using the phone, doing housework, driving, using public transportation, using everyday technology) [23, 24, 25, 26]. Accordingly, the National Institute on Aging and the Alzheimer’s Association developed new criteria—mostly ignored—for MCI diagnosis. The revised criteria operationalize the concept of “preservation of independence in functional activities” in a less conservative fashion, suggesting that PwMCI typically have mild problems in performing daily activities such as paying bills, preparing a meal, or shopping. Particularly, these patients would take more time, be less efficient, and make more mistakes when performing such activities than in the premorbid stage [27]. Interestingly, in a cross-sectional study including a large sample of patients with Alzheimer’s disease (PwAD) across different stages (n = 6209), 99.8% of patients diagnosed with very mild AD and 92.7% of patients diagnosed with mild AD according to the traditional criteria could be reclassified as having MCI based on the revised criteria [28].

The integrity of both lower- and upper-extremity motor functions is needed to perform ADL. Deficits in lower-extremity functioning (e.g., gait speed, balance, lower-limb muscle strength) have been described in PwMCI [29, 30, 31]. These deficits were found to be associated with difficulties in walking, driving, using public transports, or housekeeping, and are well-known risk factors for adverse events such as falls or institutionalization [30]. Deficits in upper-extremity functioning (i.e., visuomotor deficits emerging when performing unimanual/bimanual eye-hand movements) have also been detected in PwMCI (e.g., [29, 32, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37]). Visuomotor deficits appear to predict difficulties in eating, dressing, cooking, writing, doing household chores, and driving [38, 39, 40], thus representing potential risk factors for institutionalization and poor quality of life in the elderly [41].

Historically, visuomotor abilities in MCI received little attention in terms of diagnostic/prognostic significance, probably since apraxia (deficits in motor planning), ataxia (deficits in coordinating movements), and severe neurological phenomena have been traditionally classified as clinical signs of an intermediate/advanced stage of dementia. However, available data seem to suggest that visuomotor deficits should no longer be expected to manifest only in late-stage disease, thus deserving “clinical dignity” in MCI management.

Performing everyday activities requires not only intact visuomotor functions, but also efficient cognitive functioning. In particular, executive functions, such as working memory, set-shifting, and inhibitory control, are likely the higher-level cognitive processes most involved in planning, monitoring, updating, and finalization of complex behavioral patterns [24, 42], such as those involved in ADL. Indeed, a significant association between executive functioning and ADL in PwMCI has been reported in different neuropsychological investigations [24], although some concerns remain on whether there are specific executive subdomains more involved in ADL than others.

If one takes for granted that visuomotor and executive domains are both related to ADL, it is also true that these domains are related to each other. In MCI, the existence of such a relationship has been demonstrated in some correlational studies [43, 44], as well as through the use of experimental paradigms requiring the execution of hand movements under increasing cognitive load [45]. These findings are in line with anatomical-functional data suggesting that visuomotor skills and executive functions are both supported by neural activity in the frontal territories [40]. The relationship binding visuomotor and executive skills to functional autonomies is unclear and needs to be further explored.

In this vein, the present review aims at (a) providing a synthesis of the pathophysiological and neuropsychological mechanisms related to deficits in performing hand movements, (b) analyzing the results of cross-sectional studies comparing hand movements performance of PwMCI with that of control participants and/or patients with dementia (PwD), and (c) discussing the interrelation between visuomotor and executive abilities in connection with functional autonomies in MCI. Note that MCI in Parkinson’s disease (PD-MCI) is diagnosed when the cognitive decline occurs within the context of PD [46]. This is a well-known motor syndrome characterized by some cardinal manifestations such as bradykinesia, rigidity, and tremor [47]. Given the centrality of motor symptoms in patients with PD-MCI, an in-depth discussion of this MCI phenotype is beyond the scope of this review.

2. Neurophysiology of planning and control of hand movements

The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is an associative cortical region involved in processing and combining inputs from the motor and visual cortices, and in integrating proprioceptive and vestibular signals. These anatomical-functional relationships make PPC the pillar of visuomotor behavior. Indeed, in synergy with occipital, anterior-parietal, and cortico-subcortical motor areas, PPC plays a pivotal role in planning and controlling hand movements [48, 49, 50, 51, 52] (see Appendix Fig. 2).

During the planning of hand movements, setup of initial action kinematic parameters, including distance to be covered, trajectory, and timing, is performed. Action planning is also responsible for the selection of the target object, the way in which the object will be grasped, and the final hand position. To fulfill these aims, a large amount of information is computed, such as the metric relationships between agent and target object, the spatial (e.g., size, shape, and orientation) and non-spatial (e.g., function, weight, fragility) features of the target object, and the environment’s properties [53, 54].

In controlling the movement, the trajectory of the hand is adjusted in flight. Since agent and target rarely stay in a static relationship to each other, spatial coordinates supporting movements are updated from moment to moment [54, 55, 56]. This happens because the effector moves towards the object, the positions of the eyes and head change continuously, and the object’s location, with respect to the observer, can change quickly and in unpredictable ways (e.g., in an attempt to catch a ball). Finally, as visual scene can contain many different objects, not all relevant to the current behavior, hand movements trajectory and velocity are also adjusted in relation to irrelevant competing distractor stimuli present in the environment [57, 58].

Both planning and control of hand movements use egocentric spatial representations generated within the frontoparietal network, in which the occipitoparietal/dorsal pathway plays a crucial role [56, 59, 60, 61] (see Appendix Fig. 2). These representations, combining retinotopic and spatiotopic/body-centered coordinates, are very suitable for the needs of hand behaviors [62, 63]. Indeed, the visuomotor system exploits egocentric representations for computing the spatial position of the target object with respect to the agent’s body or body parts (e.g., eyes, head, hands) and for supporting real-time visually-guided actions in the reaching space [64, 65, 66, 67]. Some neuroimaging studies have reported a selective involvement of the precuneus, in the medial PPC, during planning and execution of visually-guided reaching movements [68, 69, 70]. Furthermore, neuropsychological evidence has suggested that patients with occipitoparietal damage typically show marked deficits in reaching objects under visual guidance (i.e., optic ataxia) [66, 71, 72, 73, 74]. These deficits may be due to the inability to perform movements according to egocentric coordinates [55, 56].

As argued before, movement is a dynamic process in which spatial coordinates guiding the action need to be continuously updated. For this reason, egocentric information can be nothing but transient in nature. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that the imposition of a short delay (e.g., two seconds or less) between object presentation and movement execution may produce a rapid decay of egocentric information, with movements being less accurate, slower, and trajectories more curvilinear than observed during actions towards visible targets [55, 64, 75, 76].

In connection with the principles regulating action planning and movement control, human hand movements are organized, under normal conditions, into two consecutive phases [77, 78]. The first phase, governed by feedforward mechanisms, covers about 90% of the movement, i.e., planning and initial motion control. It is based on preexisting internal models/representations and thus requires intact memory functions. These models encapsulate information about the sensory consequences of movements such as accuracy, direction, and speed. Almost immediately after the movement initiation, limb afference and efference—concerning, for instance, movement’s speed—are compared to the expectancies associated with internal models. In case of discrepancy between the expected and observed movement’s consequences, corrections are made for aligning the ongoing motion kinematics with the predicted ones [77, 78]. In sum, people perform hand movements mainly on the basis of their predictions. For instance, according to the Fitts’ Law [79], when required to execute a pointing movement towards a small/far target, people are slower as compared to when they reach a large/near target. This might happen because people expect, based on prior experience, that a small/far object is more difficult to reach accurately than a large/close one. Therefore, people unconsciously operate a “speed-accuracy trade-off”, i.e., they tend to sacrifice speed in order to maintain acceptable levels of accuracy.

This complex no-feedback-based adjusting process is mainly supported by the parieto-fronto-cerebellar network (see Appendix Fig. 2). The cerebellum receives a copy of the motor command from the primary motor cortex and concurrently sensory inputs from the parietal cortex. Hence, by comparing these two information sources, the cerebellum predicts the sensory consequences of the motor command and prepares the musculoskeletal system to execute the movement successfully. During the movement execution, expected sensory consequences are compared with incoming information. If a positive match is detected, the same pattern is repeated in the next movement. Conversely, in the absence of a positive match, an alert signal is sent to the frontal cortex and spinal cord for recalibration of the motor plan in line with expectations [80, 81, 82]. The detection of a discrepancy between expected and actual feedbacks has a considerable evolutionary relevance, as it may be used to correct future predictions and allow more effective interaction with the environment [80, 81].

The second phase covers about the last 10% of the movement and is responsible for the implementation of in-flight corrections by visual and proprioceptive feedbacks during the deceleration phase, i.e., when the hand is close to the target [77, 78]. More precisely, visual and proprioceptive information about limb position is compared to visual information about the target position, thus allowing further adjustments of the limb trajectory and/or movement speed in order to accurately reach the target [78, 83]. Taking the above example on the Fitts’ Law, reaching a small/far object may take longer than reaching a large/near object even because the larger distance and the smaller size probably entail a more extensive intervention of feedback-based compensation mechanisms. These mechanisms seem to be mainly regulated by posterior parietal territories, including occipitoparietal/dorsal stream [50, 84], and take advantage of intact egocentric spatial representations [83].

Overall, hand movements efficiency appears to depend primarily on feedforward/predictive mechanisms and only modestly on perceptual feedbacks. However, these two processes are not independent but rather interact mutually with each other. One might expect that hand movements are not affected by impairment in feedforward mechanisms if feedback-based compensatory processes are spared. This is not the case. For example, poorer hand motor performance in the elderly, as compared with young adults, has been associated with morphological and functional deterioration of the parieto-fronto-cerebellar network (i.e., the neural substrate of feedforward mechanisms) resulting from normal aging [85]. Similarly, patients with brain damage involving the same network have been found to be slower in performing reaching movements than control participants [82, 86, 87]. These observations underpin a selective impairment of feedforward mechanisms leading to internal models’ degradation, which results in a massive use of visual and proprioceptive feedbacks (e.g., hand vs. target position) that, by nature, are slower and less accurate than internal models for controlling and correcting hand movements [82, 87]. In a specular way, some studies on healthy subjects [88] and PwAD [86, 87] have suggested that viewing the hand prior to the movement onset (i.e., visual feedback) is needed to update internal models of the starting position, to compute the distance between starting and final positions, to plan movement direction, and to reduce movement variability and directional bias. It is therefore clear that the integrity of both feedforward and feedback-based mechanisms is necessary for planning and controlling hand movements: feedback-driven adjustments make use of information stored within the internal models to ensure speed and precision of movements; however, although internal models account for the complex geometry of the eye-head-shoulder system, their contribution to the efficiency of the eye-hand motor behavior is significantly affected by the gaze direction [89]. In sum, the human brain is able to transform a stimulus encoded at the retina level into a motor command for controlling hand movements and, in order to do that, it integrates feedforward and feedback-based transformations into one functional perceptual-motor construct.

3. Hand movements in healthy aging and MCI

Healthy aging is associated with decreased hand motor function, as indexed by difficulties in controlling and modulating force [90], reduced coordination of finger and wrist movements [91], poor coordination of bimanual [92] and eye-hand movements [93], and increased variability [91] or slowing [92] of hand movements. Possible explanations for the age-related decline of hand motor skills include reduced physical activity and sedentary lifestyle, loss of muscle mass [85, 92], higher susceptibility to distractor interference [57], atrophy and/or abnormal activity of frontal and parietal cortices, degeneration of the cerebellum, motoneuron reorganization, impairment of the dopaminergic system [85, 92], and reduced interhemispheric connectivity [94]. All these factors are involved in the etiology of visuomotor integration deficits. For instance, the loss of muscle mass may determine dysfunction of proprioceptive receptors resulting in the weakening of proprioceptive-based corrections; cerebellar structural abnormalities could lead to the degradation of internal models for feedforward control; the deviation of the gaze towards a distracting stimulus might de-synchronize the geometric organization of internal models by producing an “overestimation” of the distracting portion of the visual field, with a negative impact on the movement’s trajectory and accuracy.

Although visuomotor deficits are frequently encountered in the healthy elderly population, PwMCI often demonstrate more pronounced deficits than healthy older adults, in some cases comparable to those observed in PwD. These observations support the view of visuomotor impairment as a continuum between healthy aging and the prodromal stage of dementia. Therefore, a comprehensive and quantitative assessment of visuomotor abilities in older adults might help to discriminate between a physiological age-related motor decline and early signs of neurocognitive impairment.

To provide a synopsis of the available experimental studies comparing PwMCI with healthy controls (HCs) and/or PwD on tasks assessing visuomotor abilities, a systematic search in PubMed was performed with no year restriction by using the following string: (“mild cognitive impairment” OR “mci” OR “mild dementia” OR “early stage dementia”) AND (“motor” OR “visuomotor” OR “movement” OR “coordination” OR “dexterity” OR “hand” OR “eye-hand”). The search ended on June 2021. Eligible studies were peer-reviewed written-in-English articles published in academic journals. Conference proceedings, letters to the editor, theses, commentaries, animals studies, single-case studies, and reviews were excluded. Furthermore, we did not consider studies assessing visuomotor abilities in PwMCI-PD because of the major motor impairment or studies using self-report measures. We complemented the search by inspection of reference lists. When more papers involved the same population, the first paper in order of publication was included. Eighteen articles were selected (see Table 1, Ref. [29, 32, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37, 43, 44, 45, 95, 96, 97, 98, 99, 100, 101, 102]).

Different methodological approaches have been used for assessing hand movements in MCI, e.g., reaching tasks [37, 95, 96], visuomotor integration tasks [45, 97], the finger-tapping task [32, 34, 43, 44, 98, 99], handwriting/graphomotor tasks [33, 35, 36, 100], and the Pegboard test [29, 32, 44, 101, 102]. The following is a brief overview of the studies included in this review.

In the experiment by Camarda et al. [95], PwAD, PwaMCI, and HCs were asked to reach out and touch, with their right index finger, one of the six flat LEDs placed on a table as quickly and accurately as possible. LEDs could be placed at two distances from the starting position (15 cm and 30 cm) according to three different spatial locations: along the midsagittal plane or laterally, i.e., 60 to the right and 60 to the left with respect to the midsagittal plane. When one of the six LEDs turned on, participants performed the reaching movement towards the LED and touched it. Results showed that the movement time (ms) was significantly longer in PwAD than PwaMCI and HCs, while no difference was found between PwaMCI and HCs. The same pattern emerged by comparing the three groups on the peaks of acceleration (mm/s2), deceleration (mm/s2), and velocity (mm/s). Different results were obtained in Yan et al.’s study [96], albeit using a different experimental paradigm. PwAD, PwaMCI, and HCs were asked to hold a stylus with the dominant hand and to reach alternatively two target dots on a digitizer. The main outcome measures were the movement time and the jerk (mm/s3), i.e., the rate at which the movement acceleration changes with respect to the time unit. The jerk provides detailed information about hand movement coordination. PwaMCI were faster and more coordinated than PwAD, while both clinical groups were slower and less coordinated than the control group. More recently, Mitchell et al. [37] tested the hypothesis that PwaMCI are compromised in performing reaching movements towards objects in peripheral vision, as observed in optic ataxia. PwAD, PwMCI and HCs were required to perform reaching movements towards lateral/peripheral targets without shifting the gaze from a given fixation point or under free visual control (control condition). Although no difference between the three groups was found on the peripheral misreaching index (PMI, i.e., an adjusted index of accuracy in mm obtained by subtracting the reaching accuracy in the control condition from that recorded in the peripheral condition), PwAD showed longer movement time than PwaMCI; moreover, both PwAD and PwaMCI were slower than HCs. As the authors reported, the slowness of movements was likely due to an extended deceleration phase [37].

While performing standard reaching tasks such as those described in Camarda’s and Yan’s studies, the participant’s gaze is directed towards the target object(s) and the hand follows the gaze direction. In other terms, the target position guides the action (i.e., standard mapping). However, there are tasks involving more complex visuomotor transformations, e.g., when gaze and action plans are dissociated or when the visual feedback is specular to the motor input (i.e., non-standard mapping). For instance, to move the cursor to the top of a computer monitor (vertical plan) one may need to move the mouse backward (radial plan). In these types of experimental paradigms, the cognitive load can be further manipulated by interposing a delay between the stimulus presentation and the movement execution, thus corrupting egocentric information. Intuitively, these tasks require more cognitive resources than a standard reaching task. Salek et al. [97] examined hand movement kinematics in PwMCI using non-standard mapping-based paradigms. Particularly, participants were asked to use their index finger for moving, via a touch screen, a cursor towards visible or remembered target locations on a display monitor under different experimental conditions. As compared with HCs, PwMCI took longer to plan and initiate movements (i.e., increased reaction times, ms) requiring visuomotor transformations of moderate complexity (two levels of dissociation between vision and action). For instance, PwMCI performed worse than HCs when asked to move the cursor by using a touch screen placed along the radial plane towards remembered target locations on a vertically-placed computer monitor. Moreover, patients showed even greater difficulties, within the same experimental apparatus, when the cursor moved in the opposite direction of the finger movement. Although the authors found no difference in movement trajectory between PwMCI and HCs, a subsequent study by Hawkins and Sergio [45] reported that PwMCI were also compromised under the movements’ trajectories profile.

Some studies found that PwMCI engaged in the finger-tapping task showed altered movement rhythm as measured by the inter-tap interval (ms) [43, 99] but no difference in movements amplitude (degrees) or velocity (degrees/s) when compared with HCs [43]. Other studies reported, instead, a decreased velocity in performing the finger-tapping task. Particularly, in Roalf et al.’s study [34], the tap counting (number of taps in 60 s) was similar for PwAD and PwMCI, with both groups showing lower tap counting than the control group. A recent investigation by Hesseberg et al. [44] reported similar findings.

When required to copy a pre-established template (e.g., the Chinese characters “Yong” or “Zheng”), PwMCI were significantly slower and less accurate in performing handwriting movements as compared with HCs [35, 36]. Movements of PwMCI were found to be slower than HCs also in spontaneous drawing tasks, e.g., to draw a tree, with similar performance observed in PwD [100]. Intriguingly, PwMCI showed no difficulties in tracing pre-drawn simple geometric figures [35].

In the study by Kluger et al. [32], PwAD demonstrated an unspecific impairment involving complex motor (e.g., Purdue Pegboard assembly test), gross motor (e.g., finger-tapping speed, steadiness, strength), and fine motor skills (i.e., Purdue Pegboard and Grooved Pegboard tests). Conversely, PwMCI showed spared gross motor, and impaired complex and fine motor skills. As for the latter, performance of PwMCI was comparable to that of PwAD [32]. In a subsequent study conducted on a larger sample of PwMCI [29], both PwMCI and PwAD performed worse than HCs on the Purdue Pegboard test, although PwMCI outperformed PwAD (see also Hesseberg et al. [44]). In the same year, Lopez et al. [101] provided converging evidence supporting the hypothesis that fine motor skills (Purdue Pegboard test) are impaired in MCI. They also demonstrated that PwnaMCI-md were more compromised than PwaMCI-md; however, both groups showed greater difficulties than PwaMCI. In line with these findings, De Paula et al. [102] reported that PwAD showed reduced fine motor dexterity than PwaMCI, whereas PwaMCI-md showed a similar degree of fine motor deterioration compared to PwAD.

Table 1.Summary of the available studies providing cross-sectional data on hand motor function in Mild Cognitive Impairment.
Reference Sample Main assessment Main outcome variable/s Results
Aggarwal et al. (2006) [29] 60 AD, 198 MCI [NOS], 558 HCs Pegboard Test Manual Dexterity HCs > MCI > AD
Camarda et al. (2007) [95] 11 AD, 11 aMCI, 11 HCs Reaching Tasks Hand Movement Velocity HCs = MCI > AD
Colella et al. (2021) [43] 14 aMCI, 16 HCs Finger-Tapping Task Movement Rhythm HCs > MCI
Movement Amplitude HCs = MCI
Movement Velocity HCs = MCI
De Paula et al. (2016) [102] 38 AD, 34 aMCI, 32 aMCI-md, 20 HCs 9HPT Manual Dexterity HCs = aMCI, HCs > aMCI-md, HCs > AD, aMCI = aMCI-md, aMCI > AD, aMCI-md = AD
Franssen et al. (1999) [98] 101 AD, 69 MCI [NOS], 195 HCs Sequential Finger to Thumb Tapping Task Manual Dexterity HCs > MCI > AD
Hawkins and Sergio (2014) [45] 8 MCI [NOS], 44 HCs Visuomotor Integration Tasks Hand Movement Trajectory HCs > MCI
Hand Movement Velocity HCs > MCI
Hesseberg et al. (2020) [44] 38 PwD, 60 MCI [NOS] Finger-Tapping Task Psychomotor Speed MCI = PwD
Grooved Pegboard Test Manual Dexterity MCI > PwD
Huang et al. (2019) [35] 36 AD, 43 MCI [NOS], 41 HCs Spiral Examination Hand Movement Velocity HCs = MCI = AD
Yong Examination Hand Movement Trajectory HCs > MCI > AD
Hand Movement Velocity HCs > MCI > AD
Kluger et al. (1997) [32] 25 AD, 25 MCI [NOS], 41 HCs Finger-Tapping Task Gross Motor Skills HCs = MCI > AD
Purdue Pegboard Test Fine Motor Skills HCs > MCI = AD
Grooved Pegboard Test Fine Motor Skills HCs > MCI = AD
Purdue Pegboard Assembly Test Complex Motor Skills HCs > MCI > AD
Lopez et al. (2006) [101] 10 aMCI, 22 aMCI-md, 6 naMCI-md, 374 HCs Grooved Pegboard Test Fine Motor Skills HCs = aMCI, HCs > aMCI-md, aMCI > aMCI-md, HCs > naMCI-md, aMCI > naMCI-md, aMCI-md > naMCI-md
Mitchell et al. (2020) [37] 17 AD, 10 aMCI, 24 HCs Reaching tasks Hand Movement Velocity HCs > MCI > AD
Roalf et al. (2018) [34] 131 AD, 46 MCI [NOS], 62 HCs Finger-Tapping Task Fine Motor Skills HCs > MCI = AD
Robens et al. (2019) [100] 56 AD, 64 MCI (aMCI and aMCI-md), 67 HCs Digital Tree Drawing Test Hand Movement Velocity HCs > MCI = AD
Salek et al. (2011) [97] 10 MCI (aMCI, aMCI-md, and naMCI-sd), 10 HCs Visuomotor Integration Tasks Hand Movement Trajectory HCs = MCI
Hand Movement Velocity HCs > MCI
Schröter et al. (2003) [33] 35 AD, 39 MCI [NOS], 40 HCs Handwriting Task Manual Dexterity HCs > MCI > AD
Suzumura et al. (2018) [99] 31 AD, 15 MCI [NOS], 48 HCs Finger-Tapping Task Finger Dexterity HCs > MCI > AD
Yan et al. (2008) [96] 9 AD, 9 aMCI, 10 HCs Fitts-like Paradigm Hand Movement Velocity HCs > MCI > AD
Hand Movement Smoothness HCs > MCI > AD
Yu et al. (2019) [36] 22 AD, 14 aMCI, 18 HCs Graphomotor Task Hand Movement Accuracy HCs > MCI = AD
>, Better Performance; =, Similar Performance; 9HPT, Nine-Hole Peg Test; AD, Alzheimer’s Disease; aMCI, Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment-Single Domain; aMCI-md, Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment-Multiple Domain; HCs, Healthy Controls; MCI, Mild Cognitive Impairment; naMCI, Non-Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment-Single Domain; naMCI-md, Non-Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment-Multiple Domain; NOS, Not Otherwise Specified; PwD, Patients with Dementia.

In sum, except for a few studies finding no hand movement deficits in MCI [32, 35, 43, 95, 101, 102], the majority of experimental evidence suggests that hand movements in PwMCI are slower [35, 37, 45, 96, 97, 100], less coordinated [29, 32, 33, 34, 43, 98, 99, 101, 102], and less accurate [35, 36, 45] as compared with HCs. Interestingly, some studies have also reported a similar degree of visuomotor deterioration in both MCI and overt dementia [32, 34, 36, 44, 100, 102] (see Table 1). Therefore, this pattern of results supports the view that hand movement deficits may occur well before signs of severe motor impairment, which are typical of a more advanced stage of the disease. Moreover, part of this evidence suggests that MCI severity, according to the specific clinical subtype, might moderate the development of visuomotor deficits. Nevertheless, it is important to underline that available studies are highly heterogeneous both in terms of sample size/characteristics and assessment methods used. This heterogeneity makes it difficult to draw exact conclusions about gravity, dimensionality, and extension of hand motor deficits.

The pathophysiological mechanisms related to hand movements deficits in MCI are still unclear. Among the most convincing hypotheses are dysfunction of the frontoparietal network [29, 32, 36, 43, 95], abnormal activity of posteromedial parietal regions such as posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus [22, 37], neuropathological lesions of the pulvinar and extra-pulvinar network (e.g., superior colliculus and posterior parietal cortex) [103], periventricular white matter lesions in the frontal lobe [104], and accumulation of neurofibrillary tangles in the substantia nigra [105] (see Appendix Fig. 2 for a focus on the nigrostriatal pathway). In conjunction with these pathophysiological alterations, it has been hypothesized that visuomotor impairment in MCI is likely due to attentional/filtering or inhibition deficits [43, 103] and/or severe internal models’ degeneration leading to the selective use of feedback-based control mechanisms [37, 86, 87].

As concerns the frontoparietal network, it is well-known that it plays a pivotal role in transforming visuospatial information into intrinsic joint and muscle representations that guide the initial motor outputs of goal-directed hand movements. Interestingly, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies on PwMCI showed an early disconnection between parietal and frontal cortices likely responsible for the observed visuomotor deficits [45, 106].

Within the frontoparietal network, the PPC makes a fundamental contribution in the space-time coordination of eyes and hands movements; furthermore, it transforms visual, proprioceptive, and vestibular signals into adjusting motor commands based on egocentric coordinates [95]. In support of these claims, neuroimaging evidence has suggested a striking involvement of the medial PPC in the planning and execution of visually-driven reaching movements [22]. Net of some speculations, neuroimaging studies revealing a direct/causal link between a selective parietal dysfunction and visuomotor impairments in MCI are not currently available.

The pulvinar, in synergy with the occipitoparietal cortex and the superior colliculus (see Appendix Fig. 2), supports a wide range of cognitive/motor processes (e.g., selective attention, filtering, visual scanning, eye movements, visuospatial orientation, color and form recognition) required to properly perform different visuo-spatial/-motor activities [107, 108, 109]. A recent study, using a novel MRI-based thalamic segmentation technique [110], compared PwAD, PwMCI, and HCs with respect to the volume of thalamic nuclei, including the pulvinar. Results of analysis of covariance adjusted for sex and intracranial volume showed that most of the thalamic nuclei were significantly smaller in PwMCI/PwAD than in HCs. Furthermore, the mediodorsal, medial geniculate, and pulvinar nuclei were significantly smaller in PwMCI than in HCs.

Regular patterns of frontal subependymal lesions have been associated with decreased lower-extremity mobility in PwMCI [104]. However, to the best of our knowledge, no studies have investigated the possible relationship between periventricular leukoaraiosis and upper-extremity motor function in MCI.

Hyperphosphorylated tau protein accumulation in neurofibrillary tangles within the nigrostriatal pathway has been associated with motor deficits in the elderly with and without dementia [29, 105]. In PwMCI, clusters of tau deposition have been observed, mainly in the left hemisphere, in the frontal and temporal cortices, as well as in subcortical structures including thalamus, caudate nucleus, and putamen [111]. Interestingly, it has been reported that abnormal activity in the substantia nigra may be (i) considered a robust biomarker for distinguishing MCI due to Lewy Body Dementia (LBD) from MCI due to AD [112] and (ii) useful for identifying subjects at high risk of progression towards dementia [113].

4. Insights into the relationship binding visuomotor and executive skills to functional autonomies

As already mentioned in the Introduction of this paper, hand movements are strongly associated with ADL. For instance, planning and control of hand movements are needed to reach and grasp an object; unimanual coordination is necessary for using cutlery to bring the food from the plate to the mouth, for tooth-brushing, or for taking medications; intact bimanual coordination is required for tying shoelaces, for buttoning/unbuttoning a shirt, or for meal preparation; fine motor dexterity is needed for sewing/mending or to properly execute handwriting movements.

The relationship between visuomotor deficits and ADL has been rarely explored in both general and clinical populations. Although some cross-sectional [114, 115] and longitudinal evidence [115] exist about a decreased speed in performing ADL-related tasks in PwMCI, a few studies probed the impact of visuomotor skills on functional autonomies. de Boer et al. [116] explored the association between deficits of visually-guided reaching movements and IADL in patients with cognitive impairment (i.e., PwAD, PwaMCI, patients with mixed dementia, and individuals with cognitive complaints). In this study, participants who showed a loss of autonomy in at least two IADL domains took more time in initiating and executing hand movements compared with participants who reported no signs of functional decline. The study by De Paula et al. [102] yielded, instead, promising evidence about a selective contribution of fine motor dexterity in explaining BADL impairments in patients with neurocognitive disorders. More recently, Schaefer et al. [117] developed a simple timed coordination task to test the role of hand movements performance in predicting functional decline over a 1-year follow-up in PwaMCI. Participants used their dominant hand to convey, through a standard plastic spoon, two raw kidney beans at a time from a central cup to one of the three distal cups arranged at a radius of 16 cm relative to the central cup (at –40, 0, and 40). The time for completing 15 movements was recorded, with a shorter time indicating better performance. Interestingly, results showed that hand motor performance was a significant predictor of functional decline at 1-year follow-up for both BADL and IADL [117].

Executive functions are also related to ADL. They refer to a family of top-down mental processes (e.g., processing speed, selective and divided attention, set-shifting, interference and inhibitory control, planning, monitoring, working memory) involved in the execution of complex action schemes in order to adaptively cope with environmental requests in unfamiliar of conflicting contexts [42, 118]. Furthermore, executive functions regulate intact and/or compensate impaired cognitive domains [24]. Some studies investigating the neuropsychological correlates of functional autonomies in healthy elderly, PwMCI, and PwD have revealed a significant association between executive functions and the ability to carry out everyday tasks [24, 119, 120, 121, 122, 123, 124, 125]. As concerns PwMCI, here we provide a short summary of the most recent evidence.

In Hackett et al.’s study [126], semantic fluency test (generativity and cognitive flexibility), Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A; processing speed, visual search, and attention skills), Trail Making Test-B (TMT-B; set-shifting and inhibition/interference control), and Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT; visual scanning, sustained attention, and psychomotor speed) were found to be correlated with the Functional Activities Questionnaire (FAQ), i.e., a well-known 10-item scale designed to assess functioning across some IADL including shopping, financial management, and cooking [127]. These results were partially replicated in Nguyen et al.’ study [125] using an IADL performance-based measure. Nguyen and coworkers reported a significant correlation with the verbal fluency and similarities test (i.e., abstract reasoning and conceptualization). Furthermore, they found that TMT-B and similarities test were the cognitive measures explaining the highest amount of variance in the IADL score according to multiple hierarchical regression analysis [125]. Using a similar regression-based procedure, García-García-Patino et al. [128] found that TMT-A score was the best predictor of IADL in PwMCI.

Wadley et al. [129] calculated a cognitive composite score obtained from the assessment of several executive domains in PwMCI and mild dementia. This score entered regression models including genetic and neuroimaging markers of AD risk. The executive score was strongly associated with functional performance in MCI and mild dementia, independently of genetic risk and neuroimaging AD biomarkers. Intriguingly, by running different latent variable path models, Kwak et al. [130] suggested that executive processes mediate the neural activity of the medial temporal lobe (i.e., hippocampus and amygdala) and thalamus that, in combination with the Default Mode Network, support IADL in older adults at different degree of neurocognitive impairment.

In an attempt to root out the link binding visuomotor and executive domains to ADL, the relationship between these two domains should be further analyzed. The close relationship between motor and executive functions is predictable based on shared neural mechanisms involving cortical and subcortical frontal regions [40, 131]. However, there are only a few studies exploring this relationship in MCI.

Some studies on healthy older adults have reported a significant association between certain executive subdomains (e.g., inhibition, processing speed, set-shifting) and movement variability [132] or finger dexterity [133, 134]. In PwaMCI, Colella et al. [43] found that fewer rhythmic movements during finger tapping were associated with lower scores on the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB). The latter is the most widely used short screening battery to assess general executive functioning (i.e., semantic categorization, cognitive flexibility, planning and execution of sequential movements, sensitivity to interference, inhibitory control) in both clinical and experimental settings [135]. In the study by Hesseberg et al. [44], hand and executive functions were assessed in PwD and PwMCI. Hand functions included psychomotor speed as measured by the finger-tapping task and manual speed/dexterity as measured by the Grooved Pegboard test. Executive tasks included instead the TMT-A and TMT-B. The results showed that performance on the finger-rapping task was mainly associated with set-shifting and inhibition/interference control (i.e., TMT-B), while performance on the Grooved Pegboard test was mainly explained by processing speed, visual search, and attentional domains (i.e., TMT-A) [44].

Other research has investigated the relationship between visuomotor and executive domains through the cognitive load manipulation in visuomotor transformation tasks. In the study by Tippet et al. [136], PwAD and HCs were instructed to slide their index finger over a touch screen and move a cursor towards a visual target under four experimental conditions. In the “standard-mapping condition” the touchscreen was placed directly over a laptop monitor, and the cursor followed the finger movements. In the “non-standard mapping/rotated condition” (strategic control), the touchscreen was placed over the laptop monitor, but the finger moved the cursor in the opposite direction. In the “non-standard mapping/dissociated condition” (visuospatial recalibration), the touch screen controlling the cursor was placed horizontally in front of the monitor where the stimuli were presented, with finger and cursor moving in the same direction. Finally, in the “non-standard mapping/dissociated-rotated condition” (strategic control + visuospatial recalibration), the touch screen and monitor were placed on dissociated planes, and the finger and cursor moved in the opposite direction. Timing and performance errors were recorded. In all the four conditions, PwAD were slower in initiating and executing reaching movements compared with HCs, although the between-group differences were more pronounced under the highest cognitively demanding conditions, i.e., the rotated ones [136]. In a subsequent study by the same research group [45], a similar experimental procedure was used to compare the performance of healthy young and older adults with that of individuals at high AD risk (i.e., family history of dementia or MCI diagnosis). As was the case for PwAD, individuals at high AD risk demonstrated visuomotor difficulties with increasing levels of cognitive demand. In particular, individuals with a family history of dementia showed a selective impairment—in terms of movement variability, accuracy, and speed—in the most cognitively demanding condition, i.e., the non-standard mapping/dissociated-rotated condition. Conversely, PwMCI performed worse than HCs in all the non-standard mapping tasks. In line with these findings, Staal et al. [137] recently proposed a classification algorithm able to distinguish with 87% accuracy PwMCI from HCs based on kinematic parameters recorded during the execution of eye-hand movements with increasing cognitive demand.

Neuroimaging evidence has suggested that decreased performance in highly-demanding visuomotor tasks could be due to reduced functional connectivity between precuneus, anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, thalamus, primary motor cortex, and primary somatosensory cortex [138]. More specifically, lower accuracy/higher variability of hand movements seems to be associated with reduced functional connectivity between frontal and parietal cortex, as well as between temporal cortex and subcortical regions (i.e., thalamus and basal ganglia). Besides, decreased functional connectivity between frontal cortex and precuneus, as well as between frontal and temporal cortices, appears to be associated with hand movements slowness [138].

Finally, Lagun et al. [139] constructed an algorithm to improve the accuracy in detecting MCI by using the Visual Paired Comparison task (VPCT). The VPCT consists of a familiarization phase followed by a test phase. During the familiarization phase, two identical visual stimuli were presented side-by-side on a computer screen. After a short delay (e.g., 2 s), the test phase starts, and two stimuli, an old and a new one, are displayed side-by-side. Eye movements of the participants are recorded via eye-tracking. Typically, during the test phase, HCs spend 70% of the time looking at the novel stimulus, while PwMCI do not exhibit a significant preference for the new stimulus. This suggests that PwMCI are unable to retain in working memory the old stimulus, which results in an undifferentiated fixation pattern. These observations might partially explain some abnormalities reported in PwMCI when performing eye-hand movements. The algorithm employed by Lagun et al. demonstrated excellent discriminative capability, being able to differentiate PwMCI from HCs with 87% accuracy [139].

Overall, according to the above overview, executive functions and visuomotor abilities are related to each other; however, the direction of this relationship is still unclear. Then one question arises: how much do executive functions affect visuomotor skills, and how much do visuomotor skills affect the assessment of executive functions? On the one hand, it has been observed that hand movements deficits would emerge primarily when the task is highly demanding, thus consuming greater executive control resources [45, 140]. On the other hand, for instance, patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) commonly show executive deficits in the early stage of the disease as a result of orbitofrontal, dorsolateral and medial frontal cortices neurodegeneration; however, neurological signs of visuomotor alterations occur only with disease progression [141]. The above question assumes even more weight given that many tests assessing executive functions require a motor response. Thus, the evaluation of some executive domains, e.g., set-shifting, processing speed, inhibition, or interference control, may be affected not only by top-down cognitive mechanisms but also by manual speed and dexterity. Accordingly, future studies could further investigate such a relationship by administering executive tasks not requiring a motor response, for instance, the Stroop test [142], the phonemic/semantic alternate fluency test [143], the oral TMT [144], or the SDMT–oral version [145]. Moreover, one might consider the possibility of extracting normative data for movement-based executive tasks by taking into account not only socio-demographic variables (i.e., sex, age, and education) but also kinematic variables (e.g., manual speed, accuracy, and coordination).

In sum, hand motor and executive domains may represent independent predictors of functional autonomies in PwMCI and PwD. Since both domains appear to be related to each other, future research could test two predictive mediation models to disentangle the complex relationship binding visuomotor and executive abilities to ADL (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Schemes representing the two proposed mediation models for investigating the relationships between visuomotor skills, executive functions, and activities of daily living (ADL). The first model (upper figure) includes visuomotor abilities as mediator of the relationship between executive functioning and ADL. The second model (bottom figure) includes executive functioning as mediator of the relationship between visuomotor abilities and ADL. a = Effect of independent variable on the mediator. b = Effect of the mediator on the dependent variable. c′ = Direct effect.

5. Clinical implications and future directions

Visuomotor tasks, unlike verbally-based cognitive tasks, are relatively unaffected by formal schooling and language abilities. Thus, they represent suitable and flexible tools for investigating the presence of mild neurocognitive deficits among older adults regardless of education levels [136, 146]. Moreover, the decline in visuomotor functioning seems to be associated with cognitive [43, 45] and functional decline [117]; this makes it a dimension of great clinical interest both in diagnostic and prognostic terms.

As pointed out in the present review, assessment of hand movements can be useful for differentiating PwMCI from HCs. However, it is unclear whether the examination of upper-extremity motor function could actually discriminate between MCI and overt dementia (see Table 1). In this vein, cross-sectional studies comparing HCs, PwMCI at different degrees of cognitive impairment, and PwD are needed to determine whether and how MCI severity modulates the onset of visuomotor disorders. Furthermore, such studies could provide additional evidence about the possibility of discriminating among different MCI clinical phenotypes through visuomotor examination and about its utility in the differential diagnosis between MCI and early/mild dementia.

It is important to underline that the available data are highly heterogeneous, especially regarding the assessment methodology, which varies from classical cognitive tests to more complex computer-based procedures. As hand movements have been mainly studied in experimental settings, there is still no consensus regarding the best clinical protocol for the evaluation of hand movements in MCI. Preference should be given to flexible, no-time-consuming, and economic tools so that these can be administered within composite neuropsychological batteries during the outpatient clinical practice. For instance, the Pegboard test may be employed for assessing manual dexterity; the finger-tapping task—which can be easily performed by means of a counting machine [44]—to measure speed and coordination of hand movements.

It seems that visuomotor deficits in PwMCI are more pronounced under high cognitively demanding conditions [45, 137]. Consequently, easy-to-use visuomotor measures permitting the manipulation of the cognitive load should be designed and complemented by robust psychometric investigations, including validation and normative studies, on large samples of older adults. Particularly, phase I psychometric trials should aim at ensuring task uniformity, as well as providing a precise number of trials varying in complexity, including a clear description of the respective cognitive load. As concerns the already available tools, the traditional finger-tapping paradigm may be integrated with a sensorimotor synchronization condition, e.g., asking the patient to synchronize her/his finger movements with a variable auditory rhythm. This variant, which might be implemented in a smartphone-based application, would provide an estimate of accuracy (i.e., how much the finger taps coincide with the rhythm) and allow for manipulation of the cognitive load (i.e., from slow-to-fast rhythms). An additional promising tool is the Visual-Motor Speed and Precision Test (VMSPT) [147]. The VMSPT is a very fast pencil-and-paper task devised to quantify the speed and precision of fine visuomotor coordination. The patient is presented with a sheet of white paper on which several little circles are printed. Proceeding from the top down, the circles decrease in size. The patient is instructed to make a cross completely inside each circle, one right after the other, as fast as possible, and without skipping any circle. One point is given for each cross placed within the circle to obtain the precision score. The total number of crosses placed within, or outside, the circle constitutes the speed score. Interestingly, this task is structured to become increasingly difficult (the circles become progressively smaller), thus challenging both speed and accuracy of movements. Validation and normative studies on large and representative samples of elderly people are required to adapt these tasks to geriatric clinical practice and to control the impact of potential threats to internal validity, such as tiredness or boredom. Notably, the experimental procedure drawn by Schaefer et al. [117] appears straightforward and relatively fast. Therefore, it might be declined in the outpatient clinical practice and restructured under variable cognitive load.

From a therapeutic perspective, seeing the relationships between hand movements, executive functions [44], and ADL [102], non-pharmacological interventions focused on upper-extremity motor exercises might improve both cognitive and functional outcomes [40, 102]. Rehabilitation programs consisting of multicomponent interventions to improve both visuomotor and executive processes might have an even greater positive impact on functional autonomies. Testing the efficacy of these types of intervention, to be preferably declined in ecological settings, could represent a relevant challenge for future research. Since PwMCI may rely almost exclusively on feedback-based control mechanisms during hand movements [37], correction of any visual problems, or the use of visual aids, can be effective in any kind of training process. Note that the relationship binding visuomotor and executive domains to ADL is still unclear. Future studies could try to isolate (or adjust) the motor component during the neuropsychological evaluation of executive functions and establish whether and how visuomotor and executive domains interact in explaining the variance of ADL performance.

Finally, as outlined in our recent paper [22], no study has explored the predictive role of visuomotor deficits on conversion from MCI towards dementia: an additional starting point for future research. It has been speculated that the assessment of visuomotor abilities may be useful to early discriminate PwMCI who are more likely to convert in dementia from those who are not [96, 97]. This speculation appears well-founded; indeed, together with visuospatial working memory and metamemory (i.e., two domains significantly and individually associated to conversion [148, 149, 150, 151, 152]), visuomotor abilities strongly depend on the activity in the posteromedial parietal cortex (PMC). PMC hypometabolism is largely considered a robust biomarker of progression to AD-like dementia [153, 154, 155]. As a consequence, we suggested that clinicians focus on this tripartite neuropsychological cluster during outpatient clinical practice using defective scores as an indirect index of PMC impairment.

In the current historical period, the early detection of individuals in the prodromal disease stage is of paramount importance as PwMCI will be ideal candidates for future AD treatments. To date, 126 molecules have been synthesized, of which 28 are in phase 3 clinical trials. Among the latter, the Aducanumab—already approved in the United States—is a human immunoglobulin G1 (IgG1) monoclonal antibody that appears to be able to successfully attack aggregated soluble and insoluble forms of amyloid β, the main driver of AD pathogenesis [156]. There are ongoing studies in search of the most sensitive cluster of biomarkers for MCI diagnosis. However, the neuropsychological assessment remains the fastest, cheapest, and most functional approach, as well as the only means for quantifying the effectiveness of future drug therapies in terms of cognitive performance. Accordingly, it may be important to determine if the evaluation of visuomotor abilities, alone or in combination with instrumental examinations, actually increases prognostic accuracy, thus allowing to orient further investigations and/or preventive/therapeutical interventions [22].

Author contributions

CRI conceptualized the study. CRI, AI, and MLM analyzed the relevant literature and provided a narrative synthesis. CRI wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AI, TI and SC critically reviewed the first draft by commentaries and insightful revisions. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Acknowledgment

Not applicable.

Funding

Dr. CR Ilardi received a Ph.D. grant from the University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli” (Protocol code n. 76400; date: June 5, 2019).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix

See Fig. 2 (Ref. [157, 158, 159, 160, 161]) for detail.

Fig. 2.

Neurophysiology of the main human brain networks involved in hand movements. Visual information travels from the retina to the primary visual cortex (V1). The axons of the retinal parvocellular/P-type ganglion cells project to the parvocellular layers of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), those of the retinal magnocellular/M-type ganglion cells to the magnocellular layers. The pathway originating from neurons of the parvocellular LGN layers is specialized in the perception of colors, shapes, and details. Conversely, the pathway originating from neurons of the magnocellular LGN layers carries signals needed for analysis of speed and direction of moving stimuli, spatial location of objects in space, stereoscopic vision, and depth perception. Furthermore, it plays a pivotal role in guiding eye-hand movements. Indeed, at the cortical level, the magnocellular pathway is intimately interconnected with the occipito-parietal/dorsal stream, which is involved in planning and online control of hand movements. The dorsal stream uses visual information about the size, shape, and position of the target object to program and control movements in real-time according to egocentric coordinates, e.g., with respect to the observer's hand—Most of the efferent fibers from the vestibular nerve project towards the vestibular nuclei of the brainstem (BS), located between pons and medulla oblongata. Then, vestibular nuclei transmit information to the ventrolateral thalamic nuclei sorting it to the posterior parietal cortex (PPC). However, some vestibular neurons directly reach the cerebellum (CB). Vestibular nuclei and CB mutually exchange signals to regulate the antigravity muscles' tone and thus control balance. Note that any linear body movement or rotation instantaneously determines a concomitant movement of the visual image on the retina. Changes in visual information are also sent to the centers responsible for balance control. Accordingly, individuals suffering from a bilateral lesion of the vestibular organ might maintain balance almost normally on condition that they perform slow movements and that their eyes are both open—Proprioceptive information from muscles, tendons, and joints is largely transmitted to CB, although part of the proprioceptive signals reaches directly the PPC conveyed by the BS along the medial lemniscus (i.e., the medulla-thalamic circuit). Specifically, proprioceptive information reaches the nucleus gracilis and cuneatus and hence ascends to the ventrobasal complex of the thalamus (Th). Proprioceptive signals, together with tactile information, are integrated into the PPC, generating a conscious sense of position and movement of the different body parts in space—The CB is responsible for the muscular coordination of the distal portions of the limbs, with particular reference to hands and digits. Moreover, it guarantees harmonious progression from one movement to the next and the timing of every single movement during the execution of coordinated fine motor sequences but also when performing ballistic movements. The CB is the structural core of the feedforward network involved in the predictive phase of the action. Before starting a movement, the CB receives a copy of the motor command from the motor cortex (MC) and inputs from the parietal cortex. Based on sensory feedbacks (i.e., vestibular and proprioceptive signals) from the distal body parts, which communicate to the CB anterior lobe how the movement progresses, the CB executes a kind of “quality control” and can send rapid corrective signals to the cortex and spinal cord—The Pulvinar (Pu) plays an important role in filtering out irrelevant stimuli during the execution of movements. It has connections with both PPC and occipital cortex (OCC) and actively contributes, in concert with the occipitoparietal/dorsal stream, in performing visually-guided motor behaviors. The Pu contains retinotopic representations of the visual field and receives massive inputs from the superior colliculus (SC). The latter is involved in visuospatial attention, multisensory integration, and initiation/coordination of eye-hand movements—The nigrostriatal pathway connects the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) with the dorsal striatum, including the caudate nucleus (Ca) and putamen (PUT). SNc supplies dopamine to the striatum, which enhances the activity in the MC and reinforces adaptive motor programs, i.e., programs generating rewarding outcomes. Degeneration of the SNc dopaminergic neurons is one of the main pathophysiological features of Parkinson’s disease, characterized by rigidity, resting tremor, and akinesia. Indeed, a reduced supply of dopamine to the Ca may produce lower cognitive control of motor activity, i.e., the individual may show difficulties in responding to an external stimulus with rapid and appropriate intentional motor responses (e.g., reaching an object or regulating movement amplitude). In addition, decreased dopamine supply to the PUT may result in the inability to perform fine and sequential movements. Regarding the contents of the present caption, refer to Chieffi [157], Guyton [158], Guyton and Hall [159], Benarroch [160], and Knierim [161].

References
[1]
World Health Organization. Towards a dementia plan: a WHO guide. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. 2018.
[2]
Prince MJ, Wimo A, Guerchet MM, Ali GC, Wu YT, Prina M. World Alzheimer Report 2015–The Global Impact of Dementia: An analysis of prevalence, incidence, cost and trends. London, England: Alzheimer’s Disease International. 2015.
[3]
Peters R, Booth A, Rockwood K, Peters J, D’Este C, Anstey KJ. Combining modifiable risk factors and risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2019; 9: e022846.
[4]
Chen F, Yoshida H. Lifestyle habits and the risk factors of dementia: Evidence from Japan. Geriatrics & Gerontology International. 2021; 21: 203–208.
[5]
Killin LOJ, Starr JM, Shiue IJ, Russ TC. Environmental risk factors for dementia: a systematic review. BMC Geriatrics. 2016; 16: 175.
[6]
Sahathevan R, Brodtmann A, Donnan GA. Dementia, Stroke, and Vascular Risk Factors; a Review. International Journal of Stroke. 2012; 7: 61–73.
[7]
Kuźma E, Lourida I, Moore SF, Levine DA, Ukoumunne OC, Llewellyn DJ. Stroke and dementia risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2018; 14: 1416–1426.
[8]
Shi L, Chen S, Ma M, Bao Y, Han Y, Wang Y, et al. Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2018; 40: 4–16.
[9]
Maggi G, Trojano L, Barone P, Santangelo G. Sleep Disorders and Cognitive Dysfunctions in Parkinson’s Disease: a Meta-Analytic Study. Neuropsychology Review. 2021; 31: 643–682.
[10]
Rosenberg PB, Mielke MM, Appleby BS, Oh ES, Geda YE, Lyketsos CG. The association of neuropsychiatric symptoms in MCI with incident dementia and Alzheimer disease. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2013; 21: 685–695.
[11]
Ilardi CR, Gamboz N, Iavarone A, Chieffi S, Brandimonte MA. Psychometric properties of the STAI-Y scales and normative data in an Italian elderly population. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research. 2021; 33: 2759–2766.
[12]
Richard E, Schmand B, Eikelenboom P, Yang SC, Ligthart SA, Moll van Charante EP, et al. Symptoms of apathy are associated with progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease in non-depressed subjects. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2012; 33: 204–209.
[13]
Garofalo E, Iavarone A, Chieffi S, Carpinelli Mazzi M, Gamboz N, Ambra FI, et al. Italian version of the Starkstein Apathy Scale (SAS-I) and a shortened version (SAS-6) to assess “pure apathy” symptoms: normative study on 392 individuals. Neurological Sciences. 2021; 42: 1065–1072.
[14]
Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology. 1999; 56: 303–308.
[15]
Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology. 2001; 58: 1985–1992.
[16]
Petersen RC, Negash S. Mild cognitive impairment: an overview. CNS Spectrums. 2008; 13: 45–53.
[17]
Petersen RC. Mild Cognitive Impairment. Continuum. 2016; 22: 404–418.
[18]
Petersen RC, Lopez O, Armstrong MJ, Getchius TSD, Ganguli M, Gloss D, et al. Practice guideline update summary: Mild cognitive impairment: Report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination, and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2018; 90: 126–135.
[19]
Gillis C, Mirzaei F, Potashman M, Ikram MA, Maserejian N. The incidence of mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and data synthesis. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2019; 11: 248–256.
[20]
Galeone F, Pappalardo S, Chieffi S, Iavarone A, Carlomagno S. Anosognosia for memory deficit in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2011; 26: 695–701.
[21]
Fragkiadaki S, Kontaxopoulou D, Beratis IN, Andronas N, Economou A, Yannis G, et al. Self-awareness of cognitive efficiency: Differences between healthy elderly and patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2016; 38: 1144–1157.
[22]
Ilardi CR, Chieffi S, Iachini T, Iavarone A. Neuropsychology of posteromedial parietal cortex and conversion factors from Mild Cognitive Impairment to Alzheimer’s disease: systematic search and state-of-the-art review. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research. 2022; 34: 289–307.
[23]
Altieri M, Garramone F, Santangelo G. Functional autonomy in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type, mild cognitive impairment, and healthy aging: a meta-analysis. Neurological Sciences. 2021; 42: 1773–1783.
[24]
Gold DA. An examination of instrumental activities of daily living assessment in older adults and mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2012; 34: 11–34.
[25]
Jekel K, Damian M, Wattmo C, Hausner L, Bullock R, Connelly PJ, et al. Mild cognitive impairment and deficits in instrumental activities of daily living: a systematic review. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy. 2015; 7: 17.
[26]
Lindbergh CA, Dishman RK, Miller LS. Functional Disability in Mild Cognitive Impairment: a Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Neuropsychology Review. 2016; 26: 129–159.
[27]
Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH, Fox NC, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2011; 7: 270–279.
[28]
Morris JC. Revised criteria for mild cognitive impairment may compromise the diagnosis of Alzheimer disease dementia. Archives of Neurology. 2012; 69: 700–708.
[29]
Aggarwal NT, Wilson RS, Beck TL, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Motor dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology. 2006; 63: 1763–1769.
[30]
Eggermont LH, Gavett BE, Volkers KM, Blankevoort CG, Scherder EJ, Jefferson AL, et al. Lower-extremity function in cognitively healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2010; 91: 584–588.
[31]
Sverdrup K, Selbæk G, Bergh S, Strand BH, Thingstad P, Skjellegrind HK, et al. Physical performance across the cognitive spectrum and between dementia subtypes in a population-based sample of older adults: the HUNT study. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics. 2021; 95: 104400.
[32]
Kluger A, Gianutsos JG, Golomb J, Ferris SH, George AE, Franssen E, et al. Patterns of motor impairement in normal aging, mild cognitive decline, and early Alzheimer’s disease. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 1997; 52B: P28–P39.
[33]
Schröter A, Mergl R, Bürger K, Hampel H, Möller H, Hegerl U. Kinematic analysis of handwriting movements in patients with Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, depression and healthy subjects. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2003; 15: 132–142.
[34]
Roalf DR, Rupert P, Mechanic-Hamilton D, Brennan L, Duda JE, Weintraub D, et al. Quantitative assessment of finger tapping characteristics in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology. 2018; 265: 1365–1375.
[35]
Huang Y, Singh A, Chen S, Sun F, Huang C, Liu S. Validity of a Novel Touch Screen Tablet-Based Assessment for Mild Cognitive Impairment and Probable AD in Older Adults. Assessment. 2019; 26: 1540–1553.
[36]
Yu N, Chang S. Characterization of the fine motor problems in patients with cognitive dysfunction – a computerized handwriting analysis. Human Movement Science. 2019; 65: 71–79.
[37]
Mitchell, AG, Rossit S, Pal S, Hornberger M, Warman A, Kenning E, et al. Visually guided reaching in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. PsyArXiv Preprints. 2020. (in press)
[38]
Scherder E, Dekker W, Eggermont L. Higher-level hand motor function in aging and (preclinical) dementia: its relationship with (instrumental) activities of daily life–a mini-review. Gerontology. 2008; 54: 333–341.
[39]
Larivière S, Xifra-Porxas A, Kassinopoulos M, Niso G, Baillet S, Mitsis GD, et al. Functional and effective reorganization of the aging brain during unimanual and bimanual hand movements. Human Brain Mapping. 2019; 40: 3027–3040.
[40]
Rycroft SS, Quach LT, Ward RE, Pedersen MM, Grande L, Bean JF. The Relationship between Cognitive Impairment and Upper Extremity Function in Older Primary Care Patients. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2019; 74: 568–574.
[41]
Ostwald SK, Snowdon DA, Rysavy DM, Keenan NL, Kane RL. Manual dexterity as a correlate of dependency in the elderly. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1989; 37: 963–969.
[42]
Diamond A. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology. 2013; 64: 135–168.
[43]
Colella D, Guerra A, Paparella G, Cioffi E, Di Vita A, Trebbastoni A, et al. Motor dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment as tested by kinematic analysis and transcranial magnetic stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2021; 132: 315–322.
[44]
Hesseberg K, Tangen GG, Pripp AH, Bergland A. Associations between Cognition and Hand Function in Older People Diagnosed with Mild Cognitive Impairment or Dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra. 2020; 10: 195–204.
[45]
Hawkins KM, Sergio LE. Visuomotor impairments in older adults at increased Alzheimer’s disease risk. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2014; 42: 607–621.
[46]
Litvan I, Goldman JG, Tröster AI, Schmand BA, Weintraub D, Petersen RC, et al. Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Movement Disorder Society Task Force guidelines. Movement Disorders. 2012; 27: 349–356.
[47]
Postuma RB, Berg D, Stern M, Poewe W, Olanow CW, Oertel W, et al. MDS clinical diagnostic criteria for Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders. 2015; 30: 1591–1601.
[48]
Johnson PB, Ferraina S, Bianchi L, Caminiti R. Cortical networks for visual reaching: physiological and anatomical organization of frontal and parietal lobe arm regions. Cerebral Cortex. 1996; 6: 102–119.
[49]
Battaglia-Mayer A, Ferraina S, Mitsuda T, Marconi B, Genovesio A, Onorati P, et al. Early Coding of Reaching in the Parietooccipital Cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000; 83: 2374–2391.
[50]
Archambault PS, Ferrari-Toniolo S, Caminiti R, Battaglia-Mayer A. Visually-guided correction of hand reaching movements: the neurophysiological bases in the cerebral cortex. Vision Research. 2015; 110: 244–256.
[51]
Whitlock JR. Posterior parietal cortex. Current Biology. 2017; 27: R691–R695.
[52]
Chieffi S, Ilardi CR, Iavarone A. Parietal Lobe Dysfunction in Schizophrenia: a Review. Current Psychiatry Reviews. 2018; 14: 71–83.
[53]
Jeannerod M. The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1994; 17: 187–202.
[54]
Glover S. Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2004; 27: 3–78.
[55]
Goodale MA, Westwood DA, Milner AD. Two distinct modes of control for object-directed action. Progress in Brain Research. 2004; 144: 131–144.
[56]
Milner AD, Goodale MA. Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia. 2008; 46: 774–785.
[57]
Chieffi S, Iavarone A, Iaccarino L, La Marra M, Messina G, De Luca V, et al. Age-related differences in distractor interference on line bisection. Experimental Brain Research. 2014; 232: 3659–3664.
[58]
Chieffi S, Messina G, La Marra M, Iavarone A, Viggiano A, De Luca V, et al. Distractor interference in visual motor tasks. Horizons in Neuroscience Research. 2014; 13: 151–160.
[59]
Committeri G, Galati G, Paradis A, Pizzamiglio L, Berthoz A, LeBihan D. Reference frames for spatial cognition: different brain areas are involved in viewer-, object-, and landmark-centered judgments about object location. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004; 16: 1517–1535.
[60]
Zaehle T, Jordan K, Wüstenberg T, Baudewig J, Dechent P, Mast FW. The neural basis of the egocentric and allocentric spatial frame of reference. Brain Research. 2007; 1137: 92–103.
[61]
Ruotolo F, Ruggiero G, Raemaekers M, Iachini T, van der Ham IJM, Fracasso A, et al. Neural correlates of egocentric and allocentric frames of reference combined with metric and non-metric spatial relations. Neuroscience. 2019; 409: 235–252.
[62]
Chieffi S, Allport DA, Woodin M. Hand-centred coding of target location in visuo-spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia. 1999; 37: 495–502.
[63]
Chieffi S, Messina G, Villano I, Messina A, Ilardi CR, Monda M, et al. Hemispheric Asymmetries in Radial Line Bisection: Role of Retinotopic and Spatiotopic Factors. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018; 9: 2200.
[64]
Ruotolo F, van der Ham I, Postma A, Ruggiero G, Iachini T. How coordinate and categorical spatial relations combine with egocentric and allocentric reference frames in a motor task: effects of delay and stimuli characteristics. Behavioural Brain Research. 2015; 284: 167–178.
[65]
Chieffi S, Castaldi C, Di Maio G, La Marra M, Messina A, Monda V, et al. Attentional bias in the radial and vertical dimensions of space. Comptes Rendus Biologies. 2019; 342: 97–100.
[66]
Ilardi CR, Iavarone A, Villano I, Rapuano M, Ruggiero G, Iachini T, et al. Egocentric and allocentric spatial representations in a patient with Bálint-like syndrome: a single-case study. Cortex. 2021; 135: 10–16.
[67]
Ruggiero G, Ruotolo F, Orti R, Rauso B, Iachini T. Egocentric metric representations in peripersonal space: a bridge between motor resources and spatial memory. British Journal of Psychology. 2021; 112: 433–454.
[68]
Astafiev SV, Shulman GL, Stanley CM, Snyder AZ, Van Essen DC, Corbetta M. Functional organization of human intraparietal and frontal cortex for attending, looking, and pointing. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003; 23: 4689–4699.
[69]
Connolly JD, Andersen RA, Goodale MA. FMRI evidence for a ‘parietal reach region’ in the human brain. Experimental Brain Research. 2003; 153: 140–145.
[70]
Karnath H, Perenin M. Cortical Control of Visually Guided Reaching: Evidence from Patients with Optic Ataxia. Cerebral Cortex. 2005; 15: 1561–1569.
[71]
Milner AD, Paulignan Y, Dijkerman HC, Michel F, Jeannerod M. A paradoxical improvement of misreaching in optic ataxia: new evidence for two separate neural systems for visual localization. Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 1999; 266: 2225–2229.
[72]
Andersen RA, Andersen KN, Hwang EJ, Hauschild M. Optic ataxia: from Balint’s syndrome to the parietal reach region. Neuron. 2014; 81: 967–983.
[73]
Chieffi S, Messina G, Messina A, Villano I, Monda V, Ambra FI, et al. Memory for Spatial Locations in a Patient with near Space Neglect and Optic Ataxia: Involvement of the Occipitotemporal Stream. Frontiers in Neurology. 2017; 8: 231.
[74]
Pisella L, Rossetti Y, Rode G. Optic ataxia in Bálint-Holmes syndrome. Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine. 2017; 60: 148–154.
[75]
Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA. The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2001; 55: 304–310.
[76]
Byrne PA, Crawford JD. Cue reliability and a landmark stability heuristic determine relative weighting between egocentric and allocentric visual information in memory-guided reach. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010; 103: 3054–3069.
[77]
Woodworth RS. The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements. 1899; 3: 1–119.
[78]
Elliott D, Lyons J, Hayes SJ, Burkitt JJ, Roberts JW, Grierson LEM, et al. The multiple process model of goal-directed reaching revisited. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017; 72: 95–110.
[79]
Fitts PM. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. 1954. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 1992; 121: 262–269.
[80]
Wolpert DM, Goodbody SJ, Husain M. Maintaining internal representations: the role of the human superior parietal lobe. Nature Neuroscience. 1998; 1: 529–533.
[81]
Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kawato M. Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1998; 2: 338–347.
[82]
Bastian AJ. Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2006; 16: 645–649.
[83]
Chieffi S, Messina A, Villano I, Valenzano AA, Nigro E, La Marra M, et al. The Use of Velocity Information in Movement Reproduction. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017; 8: 983.
[84]
Sabes PN. The planning and control of reaching movements. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2000; 10: 740–746.
[85]
Bernard JA, Seidler RD. Moving forward: age effects on the cerebellum underlie cognitive and motor declines. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014; 42: 193–207.
[86]
Ghilardi MF, Alberoni M, Marelli S, Rossi M, Franceschi M, Ghez C, et al. Impaired movement control in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscience Letters. 1999; 260: 45–48.
[87]
Ghilardi MF, Alberoni M, Rossi M, Franceschi M, Mariani C, Fazio F. Visual feedback has differential effects on reaching movements in Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Research. 2000; 876: 112–123.
[88]
Desmurget M, Rossetti Y, Prablanc C, Stelmach GE, Jeannerod M. Representation of hand position prior to movement and motor variability. Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology. 1995; 73: 262–272.
[89]
Crawford JD, Medendorp WP, Marotta JJ. Spatial transformations for eye-hand coordination. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004; 92: 10–19.
[90]
Galganski ME, Fuglevand AJ, Enoka RM. Reduced control of motor output in a human hand muscle of elderly subjects during submaximal contractions. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1993; 69: 2108–2115.
[91]
Contreras-Vidal JL, Teulings HL, Stelmach GE. Elderly subjects are impaired in spatial coordination in fine motor control. Acta Psychologica. 1998; 100: 25–35.
[92]
Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB, Fling BW, Gordon MT, Gwin JT, et al. Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010; 34: 721–733.
[93]
Warabi T, Noda H, Kato T. Effect of aging on sensorimotor functions of eye and hand movements. Experimental Neurology. 1986; 92: 686–697.
[94]
Rudisch J, Müller K, Kutz DF, Brich L, Sleimen-Malkoun R, Voelcker-Rehage C. How Age, Cognitive Function and Gender Affect Bimanual Force Control. Frontiers in Physiology. 2020; 11: 245.
[95]
Camarda R, Camarda C, Monastero R, Grimaldi S, Camarda LKC, Pipia C, et al. Movements Execution in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Behavioural Neurology. 2007; 18: 135–142.
[96]
Yan JH, Rountree S, Massman P, Doody RS, Li H. Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment deteriorate fine movement control. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2008; 42: 1203–1212.
[97]
Salek Y, Anderson ND, Sergio L. Mild cognitive impairment is associated with impaired visual-motor planning when visual stimuli and actions are incongruent. European Neurology. 2011; 66: 283–293.
[98]
Franssen EH, Souren LE, Torossian CL, Reisberg B. Equilibrium and limb coordination in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1999; 47: 463–469.
[99]
Suzumura S, Osawa A, Maeda N, Sano Y, Kandori A, Mizuguchi T, et al. Differences among patients with Alzheimer’s disease, older adults with mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults in finger dexterity. Geriatrics & Gerontology International. 2018; 18: 907–914.
[100]
Robens S, Heymann P, Gienger R, Hett A, Müller S, Laske C, et al. The Digital Tree Drawing Test for Screening of Early Dementia: an Explorative Study Comparing Healthy Controls, Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Patients with Early Dementia of the Alzheimer Type. Journal of Alzheimer’S Disease. 2019; 68: 1561–1574.
[101]
Lopez OL, Becker JT, Jagust WJ, Fitzpatrick A, Carlson MC, DeKosky ST, et al. Neuropsychological characteristics of mild cognitive impairment subgroups. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2006; 77: 159–165.
[102]
de Paula JJ, Albuquerque MR, Lage GM, Bicalho MA, Romano-Silva MA, Malloy-Diniz LF. Impairment of fine motor dexterity in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease dementia: association with activities of daily living. Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry. 2016; 38: 235–238.
[103]
Chieffi S, Iavarone A, Viggiano A, Monda M, Carlomagno S. Effect of a visual distractor on line bisection. Experimental Brain Research. 2012; 219: 489–498.
[104]
Onen F, Feugeas MCH, Baron G, De Marco G, Godon-Hardy S, Peretti II, et al. Leukoaraiosis and mobility decline: a high resolution magnetic resonance imaging study in older people with mild cognitive impairment. Neuroscience Letters. 2004; 355: 185–188.
[105]
Burns JM, Galvin JE, Roe CM, Morris JC, McKeel DW. The pathology of the substantia nigra in Alzheimer disease with extrapyramidal signs. Neurology. 2005; 64: 1397–1403.
[106]
Bai F, Zhang Z, Watson DR, Yu H, Shi Y, Yuan Y, et al. Abnormal integrity of association fiber tracts in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2009; 278: 102–106.
[107]
Kuljis RO. Lesions in the pulvinar in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology. 1994; 53: 202–211.
[108]
Leuba G, Saini K. Pathology of subcortical visual centres in relation to cortical degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology. 1995; 21: 410–422.
[109]
Chieffi S, Ricci M, Carlomagno S. Influence of visual distractors on movement trajectory. Cortex. 2001; 37: 389–405.
[110]
Bernstein AS, Rapcsak SZ, Hornberger M, Saranathan M. Structural Changes in Thalamic Nuclei across Prodromal and Clinical Alzheimer’s Disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2021; 3: 1–11.
[111]
Dani M, Wood M, Mizoguchi R, Fan Z, Edginton T, Hinz R, et al. Tau Aggregation Correlates with Amyloid Deposition in both Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease Subjects. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2019; 70: 455–465.
[112]
Kantarci K, Boeve BF, Przybelski SA, Lesnick TG, Chen Q, Fields J, et al. FDG PET metabolic signatures distinguishing prodromal DLB and prodromal AD. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2021; 31: 102754.
[113]
Albin RL, Burke JF, Koeppe RA, Giordani B, Gilman S, Frey KA. Assessing mild cognitive impairment with amyloid and dopamine terminal molecular imaging. Journal of Nuclear Medicine. 2013; 54: 887–893.
[114]
Wadley VG, Okonkwo O, Crowe M, Ross-Meadows LA. Mild cognitive impairment and everyday function: evidence of reduced speed in performing instrumental activities of daily living. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008; 16: 416–424.
[115]
Lassen-Greene CL, Steward K, Okonkwo O, Porter E, Crowe M, Vance DE, et al. Mild Cognitive Impairment and Changes in Everyday Function over Time: the Importance of Evaluating both Speed and Accuracy. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology. 2017; 30: 220–227.
[116]
de Boer C, Pel JJM, van der Steen J, Mattace-Raso F. Delays in Manual Reaching are Associated with Impaired Functional Abilities in Early Dementia Patients. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2015; 40: 63–71.
[117]
Schaefer SY, Hooyman A, Duff K. Using a Timed Motor Task to Predict one-Year Functional Decline in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2020; 77: 53–58.
[118]
Burgess PW, Simons JS. Theories of frontal lobe executive function: clinical applications. In Halligan PW (ed.) Effectiveness of Rehabilitation for Cognitive Deficits (pp. 211–231). New York, USA: Oxford University Press. 2005.
[119]
Brown PJ, Devanand DP, Liu X, Caccappolo E. Functional impairment in elderly patients with mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer disease. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2011; 68: 617–626.
[120]
Marshall GA, Rentz DM, Frey MT, Locascio JJ, Johnson KA, Sperling RA. Executive function and instrumental activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2011; 7: 300–308.
[121]
Bezdicek O, Stepankova H, Martinec Novakova L, Kopecek M. Toward the processing speed theory of activities of daily living in healthy aging: normative data of the Functional Activities Questionnaire. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research. 2016; 28: 239–247.
[122]
de Paula JJ, Diniz BS, Bicalho MA, Albuquerque MR, Nicolato R, de Moraes EN, et al. Specific cognitive functions and depressive symptoms as predictors of activities of daily living in older adults with heterogeneous cognitive backgrounds. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2015; 7: 139.
[123]
Amanzio M, Palermo S, Zucca M, Rosato R, Rubino E, Leotta D, et al. Neuropsychological correlates of instrumental activities of daily living in neurocognitive disorders: a possible role for executive dysfunction and mood changes. International Psychogeriatrics. 2018; 30: 1871–1881.
[124]
Mansbach WE, Mace RA. Predicting Functional Dependence in Mild Cognitive Impairment: Differential Contributions of Memory and Executive Functions. Gerontologist. 2019; 59: 925–935.
[125]
Nguyen CM, Copeland CT, Lowe DA, Heyanka DJ, Linck JF. Contribution of executive functioning to instrumental activities of daily living in older adults. Applied Neuropsychology. 2020; 27: 326–333.
[126]
Hackett K, Mis R, Drabick DAG, Giovannetti T. Informant Reporting in Mild Cognitive Impairment: Sources of Discrepancy on the Functional Activities Questionnaire. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2020; 26: 503–514.
[127]
Pfeffer RI, Kurosaki TT, Harrah CH, Chance JM, Filos S. Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. Journal of Gerontology. 1982; 37: 323–329.
[128]
García-García-Patino R, Benito-León J, Mitchell AJ, Pastorino-Mellado D, García García R, Ladera-Fernández V, et al. Memory and Executive Dysfunction Predict Complex Activities of Daily Living Impairment in Amnestic Multi-Domain Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2020; 75: 1061–1069.
[129]
Wadley VG, Bull TP, Zhang Y, Barba C, Bryan RN, Crowe M, et al. Cognitive Processing Speed is Strongly Related to Driving Skills, Financial Abilities, and other Instrumental Activities of Daily Living in Persons with Mild Cognitive Impairment and Mild Dementia. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2021; 76: 1829–1838.
[130]
Kwak S, Park SM, Jeon YJ, Ko H, Oh DJ, Lee JY. Multiple Cognitive and Behavioral Factors Link Association Between Brain Structure and Functional Impairment of Daily Instrumental Activities in Older Adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2021. (in press)
[131]
Raz N, Gunning FM, Head D, Dupuis JH, McQuain J, Briggs SD, et al. Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cerebral Cortex. 1997; 7: 268–282.
[132]
Rodríguez-Aranda C, Mittner M, Vasylenko O. Association between Executive Functions, Working Memory, and Manual Dexterity in Young and Healthy Older Adults: An Exploratory Study. Perceptual and Motor Skills. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 2016; 122: 165–192.
[133]
Hayashi H, Nakashima D, Matsuoka H, Iwai M, Nakamura S, Kubo A, et al. Exploring the factor on sensory motor function of upper limb associated with executive function in communitydwelling older adults. Nagoya Journal of Medical Science. 2016; 78: 285–291.
[134]
Kobayashi-Cuya KE, Sakurai R, Sakuma N, Suzuki H, Yasunaga M, Ogawa S, et al. Hand dexterity, not handgrip strength, is associated with executive function in Japanese community-dwelling older adults: a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatrics. 2018; 18: 192.
[135]
Ilardi CR, Chieffi S, Scuotto C, Gamboz N, Galeone F, Sannino M, et al. The Frontal Assessment Battery 20 years later: normative data for a shortened version (FAB15). Neurological Sciences. 2022; 43: 1709–1719.
[136]
Tippett WJ, Sergio LE, Black SE. Compromised visually guided motor control in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease: can reliable distinctions be observed? Journal of Clinical Neuroscience. 2012; 19: 655–660.
[137]
Staal J, Mattace-Raso F, Daniels HAM, van der Steen J, Pel JJM. To Explore the Predictive Power of Visuomotor Network Dysfunctions in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2021; 15: 654003.
[138]
Hawkins KM, Sergio LE. Adults at Increased Alzheimer’s Disease Risk Display Cognitive-Motor Integration Impairment Associated with Changes in Resting-State Functional Connectivity: a Preliminary Study. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2016; 53: 1161–1172.
[139]
Lagun D, Manzanares C, Zola SM, Buffalo EA, Agichtein E. Detecting cognitive impairment by eye movement analysis using automatic classification algorithms. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2011; 201: 196–203.
[140]
Bangert AS, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Walsh CM, Schachter AB, Seidler RD. Bimanual coordination and aging: Neurobehavioral implications. Neuropsychologia. 2010; 48: 1165–1170.
[141]
Weder ND, Aziz R, Wilkins K, Tampi RR. Frontotemporal dementias: a review. Annals of General Psychiatry. 2007; 6: 15.
[142]
Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935; 18: 643–662.
[143]
Costa A, Bagoj E, Monaco M, Zabberoni S, De Rosa S, Papantonio AM, et al. Standardization and normative data obtained in the Italian population for a new verbal fluency instrument, the phonemic/semantic alternate fluency test. Neurological Sciences. 2014; 35: 365–372.
[144]
Bastug G, Ozel-Kizil ET, Sakarya A, Altintas O, Kirici S, Altunoz U. Oral trail making task as a discriminative tool for different levels of cognitive impairment and normal aging. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2013; 28: 411–417.
[145]
Nocentini U, Giordano A, Di Vincenzo S, Panella M, Pasqualetti P. The Symbol Digit Modalities Test - Oral version: Italian normative data. Functional Neurology. 2006; 21: 93–96.
[146]
Kluger A, Gianutsos JG, Golomb J, Wagner A, Wagner D, Scheurich S. Clinical features of MCI: motor changes. International Psychogeriatrics. 2008; 20: 32–39.
[147]
Tessman C, McDonald R. Evaluating Normative Data for the Visual-Motor Speed and Precision Test for Children [Dissertation]. Ferris State University. 1999.
[148]
Spalletta G, Piras F, Piras F, Sancesario G, Iorio M, Fratangeli C, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of awareness of illness in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment who will or will not convert to Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex. 2014; 61: 183–195.
[149]
Wood RA, Moodley KK, Lever C, Minati L, Chan D. Allocentric Spatial Memory Testing Predicts Conversion from Mild Cognitive Impairment to Dementia: an Initial Proof-of-Concept Study. Frontiers in Neurology. 2016; 7: 215.
[150]
Munro CE, Donovan NJ, Amariglio RE, Papp KV, Marshall GA, Rentz DM, et al. The Impact of Awareness of and Concern about Memory Performance on the Prediction of Progression from Mild Cognitive Impairment to Alzheimer Disease Dementia. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2018; 26: 896–904.
[151]
Ruggiero G, Ruotolo F, Iavarone A, Iachini T. Allocentric coordinate spatial representations are impaired in aMCI and Alzheimer’s disease patients. Behavioural Brain Research. 2020; 393: 112793.
[152]
Bastin C, Giacomelli F, Miévis F, Lemaire C, Guillaume B, Salmon E. Anosognosia in Mild Cognitive Impairment: Lack of Awareness of Memory Difficulties Characterizes Prodromal Alzheimer’s Disease. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2021; 12: 631518.
[153]
Cerami C, Della Rosa PA, Magnani G, Santangelo R, Marcone A, Cappa SF, et al. Brain metabolic maps in Mild Cognitive Impairment predict heterogeneity of progression to dementia. NeuroImage. Clinical. 2014; 7: 187–194.
[154]
Iaccarino L, Chiotis K, Alongi P, Almkvist O, Wall A, Cerami C, et al. A Cross-Validation of FDG- and Amyloid-PET Biomarkers in Mild Cognitive Impairment for the Risk Prediction to Dementia due to Alzheimer’s Disease in a Clinical Setting. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2017; 59: 603–614.
[155]
Ma HR, Sheng LQ, Pan PL, Wang GD, Luo R, Shi HC, et al. Cerebral glucose metabolic prediction from amnestic mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s dementia: a meta-analysis. Translational Neurodegeneration. 2018; 7: 9.
[156]
Dhillon S. Aducanumab: first Approval. Drugs. 2021; 81: 1437–1443.
[157]
Chieffi S. Dysfunction of Magnocellular/dorsal Processing Stream in Schizophrenia. Current Psychiatry Research and Reviews. 2019; 15: 26–36.
[158]
Guyton AC. Basic Neuroscience: Anatomy & Physiology. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: Sauders Company. 1992.
[159]
Guyton AC, Hall JE. Textbook of medical physiology. 11th ed. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: Elsevier Inc. 2006.
[160]
Benarroch EE. Pulvinar: Associative role in cortical function and clinical correlations. Neurology. 2015; 84: 738–747.
[161]
Knierim J. Basal Ganglia. Texas: McGovern Medical School at UTHealth. 2020.
Share
Back to top