IMR Press / JIN / Volume 20 / Issue 1 / DOI: 10.31083/j.jin.2021.01.378
Open Access Original Research
Punicalagin effect on total sleep deprivation memory deficit in male Wistar rats
Show Less
1 Cognitive and Neuroscience Research Center (CNRC), Tehran Medical Sciences, Islamic Azad University, 18116-94784 Tehran, Iran
2 Institute for Cognitive Science Studies (ICSS), 16583-44575 Tehran, Iran
3 Department of Anatomical Sciences & Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Medicine, Tehran Medical Sciences, Islamic Azad University, 19395-1495 Tehran, Iran
*Correspondence: szarrabian@gmail.com; szarrabian@iautmu.ac.ir (Shahram Zarrabian)
J. Integr. Neurosci. 2021, 20(1), 87–93; https://doi.org/10.31083/j.jin.2021.01.378
Submitted: 8 October 2020 | Revised: 21 December 2020 | Accepted: 23 February 2021 | Published: 30 March 2021
Copyright: © 2021 The Authors. Published by IMR Press.
This is an open access article under the CC BY 4.0 license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Abstract

Sleep deprivation has deteriorating effects on cognitive functions and activation of brain inflammation mechanisms has been reported by some studies following total sleep deprivation. Some studies have reported the health benefits of punicalagin, a main abstract from Punica granatum L., including those for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. The antioxidant characteristic of punicalagin and the fact that sleep deprivation accelerates mediators of inflammation led us to further explore the possible neuroprotective role of punicalagin in total sleep deprivation memory impairment in a rat model. In this study, male Wistar rats were implanted with a canula in the lateral ventricle to receive intracerebroventricular injections (drug or vehicle). The animals were trained for the passive avoidance test and then received intracerebroventricular injections of different doses of punicalagin (0.001, 0.01, or 0.1 μg/rat). Then, they were placed in the sleep deprivation apparatus for 24 hours and tested afterwards for memory retrieval and locomotion. Our results indicated that 24 hours of total sleep deprivation impaired memory processes. PG microinjection before TSD did not prevent the deteriorating effect of total sleep deprivation on memory, and only showed a tendency of restoring the memory impairment. Comparison of the locomotor activity between the animals in different groups showed a significant increase in the total sleep deprivation sham groups that received two of the highest doses of punicalagin. Considering the reported beneficial actions of PG by other studies, further investigation is needed into the possible effects of PG in memory alterations.

Keywords
Punicalagin (PG)
Total sleep deprivation (TSD)
Memory deficit
Male rats
1. Introduction

Sleep deprivation has profound adverse effects on cognitive functions including information processing speed [1] and learning and memory [2, 3]. Sleep deprivation not only leads to a decline in cognitive functions but also generates higher levels of inflammatory cytokines [4, 5]. Previous studies have indicated that physiological and biochemical morbidity following sleep disorders is related to oxidative stress [6, 7, 8]. Besides, cognitive decline following sleep deprivation has been reported to correlate with increased oxidative stress in the brain and body [9, 10].

The cycle of sleep, as a homeostatic process, is generally divided into two main phases: rapid eye movement (REM) and non-rapid eye movement (NREM) [11]. Theta waves are dominant in REM sleep and the phase is accompanied by low muscle tone. On the other hand, EEG in NREM sleep is composed of waves with high amplitude and low frequency, and the phase is accompanied by decreased muscle activity [12, 13]. Sleep cycle in rats takes about 12-20 minutes [14] and the NREM sleep-related model of REM-sleep proposes that REM sleep is homeostatically linked to NREM sleep rather than to waking and occurs in response to NREM-sleep expression to compensate certain processes of NREM sleep [15, 16]. Similar to the sleep cycles, sleep deprivation is divided into total sleep deprivation (TSD) and rapid eye movement sleep deprivation (RSD). Reports showed that TSD impaired consolidation of declarative memory [17] and motor adaptive task memory [18]. However, selective REM sleep deprivation disrupted spatial memory consolidation in an eight-box task [19] and water maze [20]. TSD and RSD both inhibit the induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) [21, 22]. Considering that REM sleep is not the only phase that is involved in the consolidation of memory [23], in the present study we selected a 24-hour TSD model to investigate its effects on memory deficit.

Punicalagin (PG), as a potent antioxidant, is a hydrolyzable polyphenol in Punica granatum L. and is a major component of pomegranate responsible for its health benefits [24]. PG also has an anti-amyloid beta (1-42) fibril aggregation effect, a potency that can be considered in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease [25]. The usefulness of pomegranates has also been reported by a clinical study for memory recovery after ischemic stroke [26]. Nevertheless, several issues central to a basic understanding of the modulation of cognitive performance and the possible prevention of memory decline by this natural herbal component remain unresolved.

The mentioned antioxidant characteristic of PG and the fact that sleep deprivation accelerates mediators of inflammation inspired us to investigate the possible neuroprotective effect of PG in a TSD memory impairment model in rats.

2. Materials and methods
2.1 Animals

Male Wistar rats (weighing 200-220 g, age 9-11 weeks) were purchased from Institute for Cognitive Science Studies and kept in standard conditions (Temperature: 23 ± 1 C; light-dark cycle: 12/12 h; lights on at 7:00 p.m.) in an animal house for one week before the experiments. The animals had free access to water and food and kept in groups of 4 in Plexiglas cages. Each experimental group included 8 rats and each animal was used only once. All the experiments were conducted between 9:00 a.m. to 1:00 p.m. under the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Institutes of Health Publication No. 80-23, revised 1996). The experiments were approved by the Research and Ethics Committee of Tehran Medical Sciences, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran.

2.2 Drugs

Punicalagin was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich ( 98% (HPLC); Cat. No.: P0023) and based on pilot studies doses of 0.001, 0.01, and 0.1 μg/rat were prepared by first dissolving PG in 0.1% alcohol (Merck, Germany, 99%) and then diluting the solution with normal saline (0.9%). All doses of PG were prepared freshly before injection. Ketamine (Alfasan, Holland) and Xylazine (Pantex, Holland BV) were purchased and used for stereotaxic surgery.

2.3 Study design

Fig. 1 illustrates the general design of the study. The explanation for each step has been provided in the respective following sections:

Fig. 1.

The experimental design. The animals that received surgery were given one week recovery period and then passive avoidance training was conducted. Vehicle or punicalagin (PG) injection was then performed and the animals were placed in the total sleep deprivation apparatus. The apparatus was either was kept ‘off’ or turned ‘on’ for the sham and experimental groups, respectively. All animals underwent a passive avoidance test followed by a locomotion test.

2.3.1 Stereotaxic surgery

Each rat was anesthetized using 2 mL/kg intraperitoneal injection of ketamine hydrochloride 10% (50 mg/kg) and xylazine 2% (4 mg/kg). The animal’s head was then fixed in a Kopf stereotaxic frame. A stainless steel guide cannula (22 gauge) was implanted above the left lateral ventricle (AP = -0.9, ML = 1.3, DV = -4.5 from skull surface) according to rat brain atlas by Paxinos and Watson [27] and fixed to the skull with instant glue and acrylic resin. After surgery, an injection of meloxicam (1 mg/kg; s.c.), as a painkiller, was made and followed by an injection of penicillin-G 200,000 IU/mL (0.2-0.3 mL/rat, single dose, intramuscular). A stainless-steel stylet was inserted into the guide cannula to prevent its occlusion during the recovery period (one week).

2.3.2 Sleep deprivation protocol

An automatic TSD apparatus (BorjSanatazma Co, Tehran, Iran) was used [28]. The apparatus has four equal-sized boxes (30 × 30 × 50 cm3) that can be used for sleep deprivation of four rats at the same time (Supplementary Fig. 1). The boxes are connected to a water tank at the bottom (120 × 30 × 50 cm3), filled with room temperature water. Inside each box, two perforated circular platforms (15 cm) move upward and downward, in opposite directions (1.5 cm/sec), using a time-controlled motor. Initially, both platforms slightly emerged from the water level. Then, each platform alternatively moved below and above the surface of the water, forcing the animal to continuously move from one platform to another at a normal pace to avoid contact with water. The speed of movement was set at 1.5 cm/sec, hence the motion cycle of the platforms was 20 sec. Once the platform was at its highest position, the rats had access to water and food pellets that we placed in a basket mounted on the top of each box. The animals were acclimatized to the apparatus for 30 min before the start of sleep deprivation period to learn how to save themselves from sinking in the water tank. Animals in the TSD groups spent 24 hours in the apparatus while the apparatus was kept on. The animals in the TSD sham groups spent 24 hours in the apparatus while the apparatus was kept off [28].

2.3.3 Intracerebroventricular injections

To inject PG, we used a 2 μL Hamilton syringe connected by a polyethylene tube (PE 20) to an internal cannula (27-gauge, terminating 1.3 mm below the tip of the guide cannula). Each injection (2 μL) was made over a 2 min period and the injection cannula was left in place for an additional 30 sec to prevent the backflow of fluid. Injections were made following the training section of the passive avoidance test and the animal was placed in the sleep deprivation apparatus after 30 minutes. The animals could not sleep during the 30-min period due to being handled by the experimenter.

2.3.4 Passive avoidance memory test

Passive avoidance emotional memory test apparatus included a Plexiglas box composed of two equal-sized partitions (20 × 20 × 30 cm3) separated by a wall, in which a guillotine door was installed to permit rat’s passage when necessary. The white partition was lit with a 25-watt electric lamp hanged 50 cm above the apparatus floor. The floor of the dark partition was made of steel rods (1 cm intervals from each other) connected to a stimulator to provide foot shocks (Settings used: Square wave, frequency 50 Hz, 1 mA applied for 3 seconds) generated by an insulated stimulator [28].

2.3.4.1 Habituation, training, and retrieval test

To habituate the animals to the passive avoidance apparatus, the animals were allowed to habituate in the experiment room for at least 30 min prior to the experiments. Each animal in each experimental group was gently placed in the white partition of the apparatus and then, the guillotine door was raised 5 sec later. As rats are inclined to move to the dark partition, the entrance latency to the dark partition (step-through latency; STL) was recorded when the rat placed all four paws in the dark partition. The gate was closed after 10 sec. and the animal was returned from the dark partition into the home cage. Animals with a STL more than 100 sec were excluded from the experiments (2 rats).

The training trial was performed 30 min after the habituation. The animal was placed in the light compartment and 5 sec later, the guillotine door was opened. As soon as the animal crossed to the dark compartment, the door was closed and a foot shock was immediately delivered to the grid-floor of the dark compartment. After 20 sec, the animal was removed from the apparatus and temporarily placed in its home-cage. The procedure was repeated in 2 min time. The training was terminated when the rat remained in the light partition consecutively for 120 sec. The number of trials (entries into the dark partition) was also recorded. All animals learned within a maximum of three trials.

For the retrieval test of the long-term memory, each animal was placed in the light partition 24 hours after the training for 20 sec, then the door was raised and the STL to enter the dark partition was measured. The test session was ended once the animal entered the dark partition. A cut-off time of 300 sec was applied for those animals which remained in the light partition. No electric shock was applied during the retrieval session [28, 29].

2.3.5 Measurement of locomotor activity

Following the passive avoidance test, the rats were tested using a locomotor activity test apparatus (BorjSanatazma Co, Tehran, Iran). The animals were acclimatized to the test room (15 min) and then put into a clear Plexiglas container box as the test chamber (30 × 30 × 40 cm3). The apparatus has a gray Perspex panel (30 × 30 × 2.2 cm3) with 16 photocells dividing the box into 16 equal-sized squares. The number of crossings from one square to another during a 5-min period was measured as the locomotion index [30].

2.4 Histological verification

To verify the cannula placement, each rat was deeply anesthetized with Ketamine and Xylazine, the brain was removed and kept in formaldehyde (10%) for 5 days, the brain was sliced (50 μm) and the cannula path into the left ventricle was verified. The data of misplaced injection sites were eliminated (Three animals).

2.5 Experimental design

Seven groups of animals were used in this study (Table 1). The animals that received surgery were given one week recovery period and then the passive avoidance training was conducted. Vehicle or PG injection was then performed and the animals were put in the TSD apparatus. The apparatus was either kept off or turned on for the sham and experimental groups, respectively. Then, all the animals underwent the passive avoidance test followed by the locomotion test.

Table 1.The experimental groups.
Group Surgery Passive avoidance training Punicalagin (i.c.v. injection) TSD apparatus Passive avoidance test Locomotion test
Normal control - + - - + +
TSD sham control + + - off + +
TSD control + + - on + +
TSD sham vehicle + + vehicle off + +
TSD vehicle + + vehicle on + +
TSD sham + PG + + + off + +
TSD + PG + + + on + +
i.c.v., Intracerebroventricular; PG, Punicalagin; TSD, Total sleep deprivation.
2.6 Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed by GraphPad Prism® (Version 9.0). Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistical test did not show the normality of STL data for all the experimental groups. Thus, a Kruskal-Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons was used to identify possible differences. STL data are expressed as median (interquartile) values. The data regarding distance traveled followed a normal distribution in all groups and were analyzed using one-way ANOVA test followed by Tukey-Kramer post-hoc test, as appropriated. Distance traveled data are expressed as mean ± SEM (standard error of the mean). P-values less than 0.05 were considered to be statistically significant.

3. Results
3.1 Effects of TSD on the retrieval of long-term memory

To test the possible effects of TSD on the retrieval of long-term memory, Kruskal-Wallis test was used on control groups data (normal control, TSD sham control, and TSD control). The test revealed that there was a statistically significant difference between all the three groups being tested [Chi-Square (2) = 11.54; P < 0.01]. Post-hoc analysis with Dunn’s multiple comparisons revealed a significant decrease in median STL of the animals that received 24 hours of TSD (TSD control group) as compared to the normal control group (P < 0.01; Fig. 2A).

Fig. 2.

The effects of post-training administration of punicalagin (PG) on: A) memory consolidation; B) locomotor activity. In this study, eleven groups of animals were used - from left to right: (i) The animals in the normal control group did not receive surgery and were tested in passive avoidance test and locomotion test; All animals in the TSD sham groups went through the procedures while the apparatus was kept off; (ii) The animals in the TSD sham group received surgery and spent 24 hours in the TSD apparatus (the apparatus was kept off) before being tested in passive avoidance test and locomotion test; (iii to vi) The animals received vehicle or different doses of PG (0.001, 0.01, or 0.1 μg/rat); (vii to xi) The animals in the TSD groups went through the similar procedures as the TSD sham groups but the apparatus was kept on during the 24-hour period to prevent animals from sleeping.
STL data (A) are expressed as median (interquartile) values for eight animals per group (Kruskal-Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons). Distance travelled data (B) are expressed as mean ± S.E.M. for eight animals per group. (Univariant one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey-Kramer post-hoc test).
** P < 0.01, as compared to the normal control group; ΨΨΨΨ P < 0.0001, as compared to the respective vehicle group.

To analyze locomotor activity, univariant one-way ANOVA test was performed. The test revealed no statistically significant difference between all the three groups being tested [F (2, 21) = 1.820; P = 0.1867; Fig. 2B].

Our data revealed that 24 hours of TSD impaired retrieval of long-term memory without affecting the locomotor activity.

3.2 Effects of intracerebroventricular microinjection of PG on the retrieval of long-term memory following TSD

Comparison of the TSD sham groups using Kruskal-Wallis test revealed that there were no statistically significant differences between the groups being tested as compared to the vehicle group [Chi-Square (3) = 6.001; P = 0.1852; Fig. 2A-TSD sham groups]. Comparison of the TSD groups using Kruskal-Wallis test also revealed no statistically significant differences between the groups being tested, as compared to the vehicle group [Chi-Square (3) = 7.68; P = 0.0471; Fig. 2A-TSD groups].

Comparison of the TSD sham groups using one-way ANOVA on the results obtained from locomotion activity tests indicated a statistically significant difference between the experimental groups being tested [F (4, 35) = 15.20, P < 0.0001; Fig. 2B-TSD sham groups]. Post-hoc analysis with Tukey–Kramer test revealed that the two highest doses of PG (0.01 and 0.1 μg/rat; P < 0.0001 and P < 0.0001) had significant effects on the locomotion (Fig. 2B-TSD sham groups). Application of one-way ANOVA on the results obtained from locomotion activity tests of the TSD groups indicated a statistically significant difference between all the experimental groups being tested [F (4, 35) = 1.259, P = 0.3047; Fig. 2B-TSD groups].

Our data revealed that none of PG doses altered memory consolidation by themselves (TSD sham groups). Also, PG microinjection before TSD did not alter memory consolidation significantly and only a tendency of memory restoration was observed. While PG changed the locomotive activity in the TSD sham group, no changes was observed in the TSD group.

4. Discussion

Lifestyle changes in modern societies have made sleep deprivation a major challenge that can increase the risk of some neurologic diseases [31]. Sleep plays a prominent role in the maintenance of mental and physical performance and inadequate sleep has deteriorating effects on cognitive functions [32] including memory functions [33].

In line with previous studies [28, 34], we here showed that acute TSD impaired memory processes in the passive avoidance task. The data also showed that PG, per se, did not have any significant effects on the memory functions in this test, but it increased the locomotor activity at the two highest applied doses. PG microinjection before TSD only showed a tendency of restoring the memory impairment and did not alter the locomotor activity. Hence, the observed tendency was not under the influence of changes in locomotor activity. Although some reports indicated no changes in locomotor activity following TSD [35, 36], other reports have indicated an increase in locomotion following sleep deprivation [37] and a recent research on the effects of pomegranate on parkinsonism proposed changes in the level of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) as a possible mechanism for the observed increase in the locomotor activity [38]. Applied methods for sleep deprivation and the duration of sleep deprivation might be the reason for the observed differences. Sleep deprivation leads to brain inflammation [10, 39], and reports indicate the protective effect of PG in brain inflammation [40, 41].

Our results showed that 24 hours of TSD impaired retrieval of memory in the passive avoidance test. Signaling pathways underlying the deficit in LTP have been investigated by some researchers. For example, the expression of BDNF and its downstream targets (Synapsin I, cAMP response-element-binding, CREB, and calcium–calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II, CAMKII) reduced in the hippocampus following 8 and 48 h of sleep deprivation [12]. In addition, the role of a compensatory increase in cAMP signaling [42] and reduction of extracellular adenosine [43] in preventing LTP deficit following sleep deprivation have been reported. Maintaining CREB at its beneficial level might be a possible mechanism for the observed tendency, as a study showed that hydro-alcoholic leaf extract of Terminalia cattapa, which also contains PG, regulates BDNF and CREB levels [44].

Sleep deprivation promotes the production of pro-inflammatory proteins both in humans and rodents [45, 46]. The anti-inflammatory effects of PG have been reported in rats [41] and its neuroprotective effect on glutamate-induced oxidative stress has been reported in the mouse hippocampal cell line, HT22 [47]. It has also been reported that PG can inhibit lipopolysaccharide-induced inflammation in certain types of macrophages [40] and anti-inflammatory property of PG works against memory deficit via the prevention of neuroinflammation [48]. Furthermore, it has been reported that PG inhibited the expression of NF-κB and the inflammatory proteins, expression of which is mediated by NF-κB in mice, resulting in the reduced expression level of TNF-α, IL-1β, iNOS, Cox-2, ROS, and NO in the brain [48]. Hence, another possibility that must be considered is the antioxidant and anti-inflammatory properties of PG.

It has been shown that sleep stages and its pattern vary in different animals, which is a point for consideration while the results are interpreted [49]. For example, other studies are reporting that sleep deprivation, for 96 hours, did not induce necrotic or apoptotic cell loss in rat’s brain [50]. Another study that measured antioxidant and oxidant markers in the hippocampus reported short- and long-term memory impairment after 72 hours of sleep deprivation, but found no significant differences in the markers compared to the control group [51]. Also, another study reported no significant rise in oxidative stress in 24-hour sleep-deprived rats [52].

5. Conclusions

PG only showed a tendency of restoring the memory impairment following 24 hours of TSD in the passive avoidance test. Considering the reported beneficial actions of PG by other studies, further investigation is needed into the possible effects of PG on memory alterations.

Abbreviations

ANOVA, Analysis of Variance; BDNF, Brain-derived neurotrophic factor; CAMKII, Calcium-calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II; CREB, cAMP response-element-binding; LTP, Long-term potentiation; NREM, Non-rapid eye movement; PG, Punicalagin; REM, Rapid eye movement; RSD, Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation; TSD, Total sleep deprivation.

Author contributions

Shahram Zarrabian and Mohammad Nasehi were responsible for the study concept and design. Mohammad-Hossein Mohammadi-Mahdiabadi-Hasani contributed to the acquisition of behavioral data. Shahram Zarrabian, Mohammad Nasehi, and Mohammad-Reza Zarrindast assisted with data analysis and interpretation of findings. Shahram Zarrabian drafted and finalized the manuscript. Shahram Zarrabian and Mohammad Nasehi provided critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors critically reviewed content and approved the final version for publication.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All the experiments were conducted under the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Institutes of Health Publication No. 80-23, revised 1996). The experiments were approved by the Research and Ethics Committee of Tehran Medical Sciences, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran.

Acknowledgment

We would like to thank the three anonymous reviewers for their excellent criticism of the article.

Funding

This project was supported by grant No. 1369704250005 received from Tehran Medical Sciences, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran.

Conflict of interest

The authors report no conflict of interest. Given his role as the Review Board Member of JIN, Dr. Shahram Zarrabian had no involvement in the peer-review of this article and has no access to information regarding its peer-review.

References
[1]
Cohen-Zion M, Shabi A, Levy S, Glasner L, Wiener A. Effects of partial sleep deprivation on information processing speed in adolescence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2016; 22: 388-398.
[2]
Feng L, Wu H, Song G, Lu C, Li Y, Qu L, et al. Chronical sleep interruption-induced cognitive decline assessed by a metabolomics method. Behavioural Brain Research. 2016; 302: 60-68.
[3]
Tripathi S, Jha SK. Short-term total sleep deprivation alters delay-conditioned memory in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2016; 130: 325-335.
[4]
Chennaoui M, Arnal PJ, Drogou C, Leger D, Sauvet F, Gomez-Merino D. Leukocyte expression of type 1 and type 2 purinergic receptors and pro-inflammatory cytokines during total sleep deprivation and/or sleep extension in healthy subjects. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2017. 11: 240.
[5]
Mullington JM, Simpson NS, Meier-Ewert HK, Haack M. Sleep loss and inflammation. Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2010; 24: 775-784.
[6]
Ramanathan L, Gulyani S, Nienhuis R, Siegel JM. Sleep deprivation decreases superoxide dismutase activity in rat hippocampus and brainstem. Neuroreport. 2002; 13: 1387-1390.
[7]
Suzuki YJ, Jain V, Park A, Day RM. Oxidative stress and oxidant signaling in obstructive sleep apnea and associated cardiovascular diseases. Free Radical Biology & Medicine. 2006; 40: 1683-1692.
[8]
Copinschi G. Metabolic and endocrine effects of sleep deprivation. Essential Psychopharmacology. 2005; 6: 341-347.
[9]
Noguti J, Andersen ML, Cirelli C, Ribeiro DA. Oxidative stress, cancer, and sleep deprivation: is there a logical link in this association? Sleep & Breathing. 2013; 17: 905-910.
[10]
Periasamy S, Hsu D, Fu Y, Liu M. Sleep deprivation-induced multi-organ injury: role of oxidative stress and inflammation. EXCLI Journal. 2015; 14: 672-683.
[11]
Stickgold R. Sleep: off-line memory reprocessing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1998; 2: 484-492.
[12]
Guzman-Marin R, Ying Z, Suntsova N, Methippara M, Bashir T, Szymusiak R, et al. Suppression of hippocampal plasticity-related gene expression by sleep deprivation in rats. The Journal of Physiology. 2006; 575: 807-819.
[13]
Greenstein YJ, Pavlides C, Winson J. Lonf-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus is preferentially induced at theta rhythm periodicity. Brain Research. 1988; 438: 331-334.
[14]
Gottesmann C. The transition from slow-wave sleep to paradoxical sleep: evolving facts and concepts of the neurophysiological processes underlying the intermediate stage of sleep. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 1996; 20: 367-387.
[15]
Benington JH, Heller HC. REM-sleep timing is controlled homeostatically by accumulation of REM-sleep propensity in non-REM sleep. The American Journal of Physiology. 1994; 266: R1992-R2000.
[16]
Benington JH, Heller HC. Does the function of REM sleep concern non-REM sleep or waking? Progress in Neurobiology. 1994; 44: 433-449.
[17]
Backhaus J, Junghanns K, Born J, Hohaus K, Faasch F, Hohagen F. Impaired declarative memory consolidation during sleep in patients with primary insomnia: influence of sleep architecture and nocturnal cortisol release. Biological Psychiatry. 2006; 60: 1324-1330.
[18]
Hill S, Tononi G, Ghilardi MF. Sleep improves the variability of motor performance. Brain Research Bulletin. 2008; 76: 605-611.
[19]
Bjorness TE, Riley BT, Tysor MK, Poe GR. REM restriction persistently alters strategy used to solve a spatial task. Learning & Memory. 2005; 12: 352-359.
[20]
Smith C, Rose GM. Evidence for a paradoxical sleep window for place learning in the Morris water maze. Physiology & Behavior. 1996; 59: 93-97.
[21]
Ravassard P, Pachoud B, Comte JC, Mejia-Perez C, Scoté-Blachon C, Gay N, et al. Paradoxical (REM) sleep deprivation causes a large and rapidly reversible decrease in long-term potentiation, synaptic transmission, glutamate receptor protein levels, and ERK/MAPK activation in the dorsal hippocampus. Sleep. 2009; 32: 227-240.
[22]
Campbell IG, Guinan MJ, Horowitz JM. Sleep deprivation impairs long-term potentiation in rat hippocampal slices. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2002; 88: 1073-1076.
[23]
Siegel JM. The REM sleep-memory consolidation hypothesis. Science. 2001; 294: 1058-1063.
[24]
Seeram N, Adams L, Henning S, Niu Y, Zhang Y, Nair M, et al. In vitro antiproliferative, apoptotic and antioxidant activities of punicalagin, ellagic acid and a total pomegranate tannin extract are enhanced in combination with other polyphenols as found in pomegranate juice. The Journal of Nutritional Biochemistry. 2005; 16: 360-367.
[25]
Das S, Stark L, Musgrave IF, Pukala T, Smid SD. Bioactive polyphenol interactions with beta amyloid: a comparison of binding modelling, effects on fibril and aggregate formation and neuroprotective capacity. Food & Function. 2016; 7: 1138-1146.
[26]
Bellone JA, Murray JR, Jorge P, Fogel TG, Kim M, Wallace DR, et al. Pomegranate supplementation improves cognitive and functional recovery following ischemic stroke: a randomized trial. Nutritional Neuroscience. 2018; 22: 738-743.
[27]
George P, Charles W. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates (pp. 32). Qingchuan Zhuge translate. Beijing: People’s Medical Publishing House. 2007.
[28]
Norozpour Y, Nasehi M, Sabouri-Khanghah V, Torabi-Nami M, Zarrindast MR. The effect of CA1 alpha2 adrenergic receptors on memory retention deficit induced by total sleep deprivation and the reversal of circadian rhythm in a rat model. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2016; 133: 53-60.
[29]
Najar F, Nasehi M, Haeri-Rohani S, Zarrindast M. The involvement of medial septum 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptors on ACPA-induced memory consolidation deficit: possible role of TRPC3, TRPC6 and TRPV2. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2015; 29: 1200-1208.
[30]
Nasehi M, Ketabchi M, Khakpai F, Zarrindast M. The effect of CA1 dopaminergic system in harmaline-induced amnesia. Neuroscience. 2015; 285: 47-59.
[31]
Palma J, Urrestarazu E, Iriarte J. Sleep loss as risk factor for neurologic disorders: a review. Sleep Medicine. 2013; 14: 229-236.
[32]
Dutil C, Walsh JJ, Featherstone RB, Gunnell KE, Tremblay MS, Gruber R, et al. Influence of sleep on developing brain functions and structures in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2018; 42: 184-201.
[33]
Killgore WDS. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Progress in Brain Research. 2010; 29: 105-129.
[34]
Eydipour Z, Vaezi G, Nasehi M, Haeri-Rouhani S, Zarrindast M. Different role of CA1 5HT3 serotonin receptors on memory acquisition deficit induced by total (TSD) and REM Sleep Deprivation (RSD). Archives of Iranian Medicine. 2017; 20: 581-588.
[35]
Onaolapo JO, Onaolapo YA, Akanmu AM, Olayiwola G. Caffeine and sleep-deprivation mediated changes in open-field behaviours, stress response and antioxidant status in mice. Sleep Science. 2016; 9: 236-243.
[36]
Morzelle MC, Salgado JM, Telles M, Mourelle D, Bachiega P, Buck HS, et al. Neuroprotective effects of pomegranate peel extract after chronic infusion with amyloid-beta peptide in mice. PLoS ONE. 2016; 11: e0166123.
[37]
de Souza L, Andersen ML, Smaili SS, Lopes GS, Ho PS, Papale LA, et al. Age-related changes during a paradigm of chronic sleep restriction. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010; 214: 201-205.
[38]
Ali A, Kamal M, Khalil M, Ali S, Elariny H, Bekhit A, et al. Behavioral, biochemical and histopathological effects of standardised pomegranate extract with vinpocetine, propolis or cocoa in a rat model of Parkinson’s disease. 2020. (in preparation)
[39]
Manchanda S, Singh H, Kaur T, Kaur G. Low-grade neuroinflammation due to chronic sleep deprivation results in anxiety and learning and memory impairments. Molecular and Cellular Biochemistry. 2018; 449: 63-72.
[40]
Xu X, Yin P, Wan C, Chong X, Liu M, Cheng P, et al. Punicalagin inhibits inflammation in LPS-induced RAW264.7 macrophages via the suppression of TLR4-mediated MAPKs and NF-kappaB activation. Inflammation. 2014; 37: 956-965.
[41]
Lin CC, Hsu YF, Lin TC. Effects of punicalagin and punicalin on carrageenan-induced inflammation in rats. The American Journal of Chinese Medicine. 1999; 27: 371-376.
[42]
Vecsey CG, Baillie GS, Jaganath D, Havekes R, Daniels A, Wimmer M, et al. Sleep deprivation impairs cAMP signalling in the hippocampus. Nature. 2009; 461: 1122-1125.
[43]
Alhaider IA, Aleisa AM, Tran TT, Alkadhi KA. Caffeine prevents sleep loss-induced deficits in long-term potentiation and related signaling molecules in the dentate gyrus. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2010; 31: 1368-1376.
[44]
Chandrasekhar Y, Ramya EM, Navya K, Phani Kumar G, Anilakumar KR. Antidepressant like effects of hydrolysable tannins of Terminalia catappa leaf extract via modulation of hippocampal plasticity and regulation of monoamine neurotransmitters subjected to chronic mild stress (CMS). Biomedicine & Pharmacotherapy. 2017; 86: 414-425.
[45]
Wright KP, Drake AL, Frey DJ, Fleshner M, Desouza CA, Gronfier C, et al. Influence of sleep deprivation and circadian misalignment on cortisol, inflammatory markers, and cytokine balance. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2015; 47: 24-34.
[46]
Chennaoui M, Gomez-Merino D, Drogou C, Geoffroy H, Dispersyn G, Langrume C, et al. Effects of exercise on brain and peripheral inflammatory biomarkers induced by total sleep deprivation in rats. Journal of Inflammation. 2015; 12: 56.
[47]
Pathakoti K, Goodla L, Manubolu M, Tencomnao T. Metabolic alterations and the protective effect of punicalagin against glutamate-induced oxidative toxicity in HT22 cells. Neurotoxicity Research. 2017; 31: 521-531.
[48]
Kim YE, Hwang CJ, Lee HP, Kim CS, Son DJ, Ham YW, et al. Inhibitory effect of punicalagin on lipopolysaccharide-induced neuroinflammation, oxidative stress and memory impairment via inhibition of nuclear factor-kappaB. Neuropharmacology. 2017; 117: 21-32.
[49]
Prince T, Abel T. The impact of sleep loss on hippocampal function. Learning & Memory. 2013; 20: 558-569.
[50]
Hipólide DC, D’Almeida V, Raymond R, Tufik S, Nobrega JN. Sleep deprivation does not affect indices of necrosis or apoptosis in rat brain. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2002; 112: 155-166.
[51]
Nabaee E, Kesmati M, Shahriari A, Khajehpour L, Torabi M. Cognitive and hippocampus biochemical changes following sleep deprivation in the adult male rat. Biomedicine & Pharmacotherapy. 2018; 104: 69-76.
[52]
M Melgarejo-Gutiérrez M, Acosta-Peña E, Venebra-Muñoz A, Escobar C, Santiago-García J, Garcia-Garcia F. Sleep deprivation reduces neuroglobin immunoreactivity in the rat brain. Neuroreport. 2013; 24: 120-125.
Share
Back to top