IMR Press / JIN / Volume 21 / Issue 6 / DOI: 10.31083/j.jin2106159
Open Access Original Research
The Evaluation Indexes Suitable for Nonhuman Primates can be Extracted from Clinical Consciousness Disorder Assessment Scales: A Hypothesis
Wei-ming Sun1,2,3,4,*,†Guan-xiu Liu3,4,†Chang-hao Le3,4Can Li1,2Xiang-li Dong4,5Chao-lin Ma1,2,*
Show Less
1 School of Life Science, Nanchang University, 330031 Nanchang, Jiangxi, China
2 Institute of Life Science, Nanchang University, 330031 Nanchang, Jiangxi, China
3 Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, The First Affiliated Hospital of Nanchang University, 330036 Nanchang, Jiangxi, China
4 Jiangxi Medical College, Nanchang University, 330031 Nanchang, Jiangxi, China
5 Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, The Second Affiliated Hospital of Nanchang University, 330006 Nanchang, Jiangxi, China
*Correspondence: sunweiming@email.ncu.edu.cn (Wei-ming Sun); chaolinma@ncu.edu.cn (Chao-lin Ma)
These authors contributed equally.
Academic Editor: Sergio Bagnato
J. Integr. Neurosci. 2022, 21(6), 159; https://doi.org/10.31083/j.jin2106159
Submitted: 22 April 2022 | Revised: 1 June 2022 | Accepted: 9 June 2022 | Published: 21 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Disorders of Consciousness: From Pathophysiology to Treatment)
Copyright: © 2022 The Author(s). Published by IMR Press.
This is an open access article under the CC BY 4.0 license.
Abstract

Background: Currently, case studies or clinical trials in different patient populations remain the main resource underlying the understanding of disorder of consciousness (DoC). This provides a low efficacy for the derivation of data and the implementation of associated controlled experimental designs. Preclinical models provide precise controls, reduced variability, rich data output and limited ethical complexity. Nonhuman primates are suitable model animals for disorders of consciousness due to their brain structure being very similar to that of humans. Behavioral tests remain the primary standard for assessing the consciousness status of humans. However, there is currently no behavioral assessment scale available for evaluation of the state of consciousness disorder in nonhuman primates. This presents a significant challenge for the establishment of different models of consciousness disorder. Therefore, there is considerable motivation to focus on the development of a proper tool for assessment of the state of consciousness associated with nonhuman primate models that are based on clinically common consciousness assessment scales. Methods: It is assumed that the Delphi and level analysis methods based on clinical consciousness disorder assessment scales may provide an effective way to select and include assessment indexes for levels of consciousness in nonhuman primates. Results: 8 first-level indicators with 41 second-level indexes were selected preliminary as a pool of evaluation entries of state of consciousness of nonhuman primates. Conclusions: It may be practicable to extract appropriate indicators for non-human primates from the clinical consciousness disorder assessment scales. Besides, a combination of Delphi method, behavioral analysis, electroencephalography, neuroimaging (such as positron emission tomography-computed tomography) and functional magnetic resonance imaging is necessary to test the reliability and validity of the novel scale reported here.

Keywords
assessment scale
disorders of consciousness
nonhuman primates
animal models
1. Introduction

Consciousness is a very vague concept with no widely accepted definition. Generally, it can be divided into two aspects: (1) Content-related consciousness (i.e., the local state) and (2) The awakening state (i.e., the global state) [1, 2, 3, 4]. Local states of consciousness include various perceptual experiences, for example, image, affect, body sensations and current thoughts. In the science of consciousness, local states are often referred to as contents of consciousness because they are usually distinguished from others by the objects and features they represent. However, global states of consciousness are not usually distinguished from one another based on the objects or features represented in experience. Rather, they are typically differentiated on the basis of cognitive, behavioral, and physiological factors [5]. The ability to stay awake and awareness of the environment are important characteristics of connection among conscious individuals [6].

Recently, with the rapid development of modern neuroscience and brain science, new technologies and strategies have been developed for the study of human and animal functions of consciousness. For this reason, the 2012 Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness was a turning point in the history of animal consciousness, stating in clear language that nonhuman animals have a similar state of consciousness to humans. Animals like humans, can sense, feel pain and have emotional responses [7]. Here, the main concern is the characteristics of consciousness in nonhuman primates. Ben-Haim et al. [8] used an elegant and simple visual counter-cuing task that found striking similarities between human and monkey behavior. The task required the subjects to shift their eyes from visual cues to reward goals. When the cue was displayed for 250 milliseconds, both subjects easily took their eyes off the cue. Conversely, when the cue was presented so briefly that the human reported not seeing it, neither human nor animal learned to look away and the behavior changed in the opposite direction. The cue captured attention, slightly increasing the amount of time the subjects looked at the reward location opposite the cue. Ben-Haim et al. [8] suggested that monkeys have both conscious and unconscious visual perception patterns based on the similarity between human and monkey eye movements. Conscious behavior suggests that nonhuman primate consciousness can be expressed as phenomenal consciousness. Being able to identify the self in a mirror is thought to be an effective means of verifying self-awareness. Chang et al. [9] found that trained rhesus monkeys spontaneously displayed the ability of mirror self-recognition through a new method of visual ontology position coupling training. Nonhuman primates do not have the ability to use language to express mental feelings. Some researchers have come up with ways to talk to them, with some remarkable results. Some researchers used hand gestures to communicate with them, while others simply spoke to them in American pantomime. The most interesting thing is to talk via the language of a keyboard, which has more than 200 keys, each printed with a different geometric pattern, one pattern representing a word or number. Chimps quickly learned to use this language system to communicate and express their needs [10]. This suggests that nonhuman primates are capable of perceiving external objects and associating with themselves, which is temporarily classified as access consciousness. Nonhuman primates show different awakening states in different brain injury states or different levels of anesthesia and they show different levels of loss of consciousness, as do humans. So, it is concluded feasible to use nonhuman primates as models for studying consciousness.

Nowadays, the survival rate of patients with severe brain injury and stroke has been greatly improved and there has been a large increase in the number of patients with clinical disorders of consciousness [11]. In the United States, approximately 100,000 to 300,000 people have been diagnosed with prolonged DoC, while in Europe the prevalence varies from 0.2 to 6.1 people per 100,000. There is no accurate data on prolonged DoC in China, but it is believed to have increased progressively there over time [12]. These people pose an extremely heavy burden on their families and society. For such reasons, there is strong motivation to seek improved treatments for consciousness disorders.

In clinical practice, recovery of consciousness (ROC) is rigorously assessed, including examination of cortical activity [13], spontaneous behavior and response to stimuli [14]. However, the detailed underlying mechanisms that support these changes remain unknown. Additionally, the lack of such information ultimately limits physicians in the accurate determination of levels of consciousness, which has a negative influence on making treatment plans to assess prognosis and estimated time to recovery from coma [15]. Currently, the understanding of DoC is largely dependent on individual case studies or clinical trials in different patient populations, which have limited ability to export data and implement controlled experimental designs [16]. It is predicted that preclinical models will provide precise control with reduced variability, rich data output and limited ethical complexity. Therefore, using animal models to elucidate unknown mechanisms appears to be the best choice. Rodents play a vital role in the study of disorders of consciousness, but they do not fully explain the mechanisms of disorders of consciousness and it is likely not possible to model certain types of DoCs (such as diffuse axonal injury) [17]. Nonhuman primates have a developmental pathway similar to that of humans in terms of anatomy, physiology, genetics, neurological function as well as their cognitive, emotional and social behavior [18]. Therefore, NHPs have a unique advantage in modeling DoCs.

Although objective techniques such as electroencephalography (EEG) [19], neuroimaging (e.g., positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) [20]) can be used to evaluate consciousness of substitutes. The American Academy of Neurology and the American Clinical Neurophysiology Society define quantitative EEG as: “…the mathematical processing of digitally recorded EEG in order to highlight specific waveform components, transform the EEG into a format or domain that elucidates relevant information, or associate numerical results with the EEG data for subsequent review or comparison” [21]. Spectral power analysis is a standard EEG method, increased low power (δ and θ) and decreased high power (α) in resting EEG are common spectral features in DoC patients [22]. Evoked potentials are “electrical manifestations of the brain’s reception of and response to an external stimulus” [21]. Several EEG markers, such as somatosensory evoked potential (SEP), auditory evoked potential (AEP) and event-related potential (ERP), are used in clinical routine and can be used to evaluate the prognosis of patients with DoC [23]. Functional imaging is able to detect local activity in different brain regions and explore their interactions. Resting-state fMRI and PET-CT analyses of whole-brain multiple network activation and connectivity strength add information to assessment and prognosis [24], however, fMRI and PET-CT studies easily produce motion artifacts, and patients with consciousness disorders have difficulty not moving limb parts, which causes problems with the evaluation of consciousness. Also, MR scanners are uncomfortable for patients with DoC. As a result, because these techniques are expensive, time-consuming and inconvenient to operate, behavioral test remains the best method for assessing human consciousness [25].

Robust operationally defined behavioral markers have been developed to distinguish conscious behaviors in clinical evaluation, particularly those scales that discriminate coma from related disorders of consciousness [26, 27]. However, progress in the assessment of levels of consciousness in nonhuman primates is surprisingly limited compared to the quantitative behavioral assessments used for patient evaluations [28]. When Gennarelli et al. [29] constructed a diffuse axonal injury model of nonhuman primates, they found it difficult to describe the severity of coma. Systems describing human comas are not satisfactory for laboratory animals. For example, the Glasgow Coma Scale cannot be used because it relies on language elements and the assessment of response to motor commands. So a behavioral scale was developed to describe the severity of the coma. Loss of consciousness (LoC) may occur in sleep anesthesia and pathological states. The reconstruction of brain function after consciousness interruption is similar to the recovery of simulated pathological states, so there is positive clinical significance in its study. It is difficult to study the mechanism of the process of recovery of consciousness in both the unpredictability of the recovery of pathological state and the rapidity of the recovery of sleep. General anesthesia is a controllable and repeatable intervention, so the recovery process can be systematically studied by influencing consciousness under anesthesia. Tassie et al. [30] and Redinbaugh et al. [31] have developed evaluation indexes for the anesthetic awakening behavior of nonhuman primates which are based on clinical assessment scales for evaluation of the level of consciousness in nonhuman primates. However, this must be studied further if such evaluation indexes are to have credibility. Alternatively, there is currently no assessment method to test the level of consciousness in nonhuman primate models, which means it is a significant challenge to establish animal models at different levels of consciousness. Therefore, it is necessary to develop a behavioral scale for the evaluation of consciousness disorders in nonhuman primates. Having objective indicators will also allow meaningful comparisons across studies and optimize preclinical results which, as a consequence, are more likely to be successfully translated from the laboratory to the clinic. This will eventually improve the testing of treatments and help in the accurate diagnosis of disorders of consciousness and brain damage. It also helps to elucidate the detailed underlying mechanisms behind the clinical consciousness assessment of behavior scale.

2. Hypothesis

Nonhuman primates are very similar to humans in terms of genetic, neuroanatomical, cognitive and behavioral characteristics [18]. The authors’ efforts are focused on the development of a suitable tool for assessing levels of consciousness in nonhuman primate models based on a clinically typically consciousness assessment scale.

3. Evaluation of the Hypothesis

The aim is to form a behavioral scale to evaluate levels of consciousness in nonhuman primates, in the expectation that it will assist in progression of verification of the foregoing hypothesis. Meanwhile, it is proposed that the Delphi technique and level analysis method may be used as an effective way to select and include assessment indexes for the level of consciousness in nonhuman primates. Finally, for nonhuman primate model animals with disorders of consciousness, it is proposed that a combination of behavioral analysis and fMRI, EEG and PET-CT will suitably test the reliability and validity the of scale developed.

3.1 Establishment of Evaluation Indexes

The Delphi technique [32] is a research method that aims to achieve a consensus judgment of experts. The consultation of such experts facilitates the development and prioritization of areas that can lead to actions for the resolution of social issues. The technique is widely used across many specialist professions working in social contexts [33] and is one comprised of five essential stages [34]:

(1) Constitution of an Expert Group;

(2) Establishment of a Pool of Evaluation Entries;

(3) Develop Expert Correspondence Questionnaire;

(4) Expert Selection and Consultation;

(5) Statistics Analysis.

The objective is to determine the consensus of expert opinion and reach consensus on the topic being studied. This is a very useful technique for pooling expert opinion on a topic to support innovative thinking or change in areas including health and rehabilitation [35]. The process could take two to four rounds, depending on the number of responses and the level of consensus. In the Delphi study, consensus was defined as greater than 70% agreement on all items with the same ranking [36].

3.1.1 Constitution of an Expert Group

An expert group should include professors major in neurobiology, neurology, neurosurgery, rehabilitation medicine, anesthesiology and veterinary medicine. The functions of an expert group are as follows: literature reviewing, preparation of expert letter consultation item pool, selection of expert letter consultation questionnaire, distribution and recycling, analysis and discussion of expert opinions, as well as the establishment of a nonhuman primate consciousness disorder evaluation index system.

3.1.2 Establishment of a Pool of Evaluation Entries

(1) Literature Search

By use of the resources of the Nanchang University Library, the following databases were selected: PubMed, Web of Science, Elsevier Science Direct, China National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI), Wanfang Data, China Science and Technology Journal Database (VIP) and the Chinese biomedical database (CBM). A literature search identified articles published as of 1 March 2022. A comprehensive literature search on consciousness assessment tools for nonhuman primates and humans was conducted, as well as similar studies in rodents, dogs, horses and other animals were searched.

Thirteen primary search terms were used to define DoC: apallic syndrome, akinetic mutism, coma AND post-head injury, coma AND vegetative state, coma AND posttraumatic, coma AND post-trauma, minimally conscious, coma AND traumatic, persistent vegetative state, minimally responsive, prolonged post-traumatic unawareness, post-head injury coma and unawareness state. Each of the 13 primary terms was paired with 30 secondary terms that defined aspects of measurement: classification, assessment, course, diagnosis, evaluation, diagnostic, injury severity score, measure, instrument, natural history, observer variation, neurologic examination, outcome, predictive, prognostic, prognosis, progression, questionnaire, psychometric, recovery, reliability, scale, reproducibility of results, sensitivity, specificity, tool, test, trauma severity indices, validity and validation. Filter terms (e.g., animal, plant, ethics, religion) were used to eliminate irrelevant articles. Additional searches were conducted using scale names, abbreviations and author names. Finally, task force members used personal knowledge of DoC scale articles and examined references in reviewed articles to identify additional relevant articles. Finally, 27 scales were screened out, as shown in Table 1.

Table 1.List of clinical consciousness disorders assessment scales based on literature search.
No. Scale Evaluation index
1 Rancho Los Amigos Level of Cognitive Functioning Responsiveness (I–X)
2 Glasgow Coma Scale Eye, verbal, motor
3 Edinburgh-2 Coma Scale Pain, cognition
4 Comprehensive Level of Consciousness Scale Eye responses, motor, posture, communication, general responsiveness
5 Glasgow-Liege Scale Eye, verbal, motor, brainstem reflexes
6 Western Neuro Sensory Stimulation Profile Visual, tactile, olfactory, arousal/attention, expressive communication, auditory, vocalization
7 Innsbruck Coma Scale Eye responses, auditory, pain, posture, oral
8 Coma Near Coma Scale Visual, auditory, command following, threat response, olfactory, tactile, pain, vocalization
9 Sensory Stimulation Assessment Measure Auditory, vision, tactile, olfactory, gustatory, eye-opening, motor, vocalization
10 Chinese Vegetative State Scale Motor, eye response, eating, emotional responses, command following, verbal
11 Coma Exit Chart Facial expression, sensory functions: vision, auditory, tactile, motor abilities: eye-opening, head control, arm and hand control, leg control, vocalization
12 The Maryland Psychiatric Research Center Involuntary Movement Scale Motor
13 Preliminary Neuropsychological Battery Verification tasks
14 Sensory Modality Assessment Rehabilitation Technique Auditory, vision, tactile, olfactory, gustatory, wakefulness, motor, communication
15 Wessex Head Injury Matrix Basic behaviors, social/communication, attention/ cognitive, orientation/ memory
16 Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination Level of consciousness, orientation, attention, language, constructional praxis, memory, calculations, reasoning
17 Putney Auditory Comprehension Screening Test Auditory comprehension screening test
18 Loewenstein Communication Scale Mobility, respiration, visual, auditory, communication
19 Coma Recovery Scale-Revised Scale Auditory, visual, motor, oral, communication, arousal
20 Full Outline of Un-responsiveness Eye response, motor response, respiration, brainstem reflexes
21 Disorders of Consciousness Scale Auditory, visual, tactile, sensory, swallowing, olfactory
22 Nociception Coma Scale-Revised Motor response, verbal response, facial expression
23 Chinese Vegetative State Scale Motor, eye response, auditory function, eating, emotional response
24 Adelaide Paediatric Coma Scale Eye, verbal, motor
25 Steward Awakening Score Consciousness, respiratory tract, physical activity
26 Modified Aldrete Recovery Score Activity, respiratory, circulation, consciousness, O2 saturation, pain
27 Glasgow-Pittsburgh score Eye, verbal, motor, pupil, cranial nerve reflexes, seizures, spontaneous breathing

(2) Extract Evaluation Indexes

To better screen evaluation indicators the 27 behavioral scales for clinical evaluation of patient consciousness given in Table 1 were employed, including the grading standards of evaluation indicators and the operation process of evaluation indicators. Indicators were further classified and summarized in each scale from the dimensions of the evaluation indicators with the most frequently used one selected to help find the commonalities of these scales. These high frequency indicators will likely also be applied to the assessment of consciousness in nonhuman primates. Generally, first and second-level indicators were extracted and classified into 27 scales and the scale to which each indicator belonged was marked according to the number in Table 1, as shown in Table 2.

Table 2.Classification of evaluation indexes.
First-level indexes Second-level indexes The number in Table 1
Sensory Visual 6, 8, 11, 14, 15, 18, 19, 21
Auditory 6, 7, 8, 11, 14, 17, 18, 19, 21, 23
Tactile 8, 11, 14, 21
Olfactory 8, 14, 21
Gustatory 14, 21
Vestibular sensation 21
Pain 3, 7, 8, 26
Motor Eye 2, 4, 5, 7, 9, 10, 11, 20, 23, 24, 27
Oral/EatingSwallowing 10, 19, 21, 23
Limbs 2, 4, 5, 9, 10, 11, 12, 14, 19, 20, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27
Posture 4, 7
Face 11, 22
Head 11
Arms/Hands 11
Leg 11
Verbal Vocalization 9,11
Verbal 2, 5, 7, 8, 10,16, 18, 19, 22, 24, 27
Communication 4, 6, 14, 15, 19
Social 1, 15
Perceptual Perceptual 3, 13, 15, 16, 17, 21
Emotional response 10, 23
Command following 8,10
Constructional praxis 16, 21
Functional object application 6, 21
Brainstem reflexes Pupil reflex 6, 7, 21, 22, 27
Corneal reflex 20, 27
Cough reflex 20
Fronto-orbicular reflex 5
Vertical oculocephalic reflex. 5, 27
Horizontal oculocephalic reflex 5
Oculo-cardiac reflex 5
Blink reflex 18
Vital sign Respiration 5, 18, 20, 21, 23, 26, 27
Heart rate 21, 23
Muscular tension 12
Pupillary size 7, 21
Circulation 23, 26
O2 Saturation 26
Nausea/vomiting 26
Sleep-wake cycle 23
Arousal 6, 14, 19, 25
Facial expression 11, 22
Attention 6, 15
Mobility 18
Threat response 8
Seizures 27

(3) Selected Evaluation Indexes

To obtain the comprehensive evaluation index system developed here a large number of articles about the physiological characteristics of nonhuman primates and the anesthesia awakening behavior of nonhuman primates were read. The appropriate indicators for evaluating the consciousness level of the model animal were then selected from the indicators in Table 2. Finally, 8 preliminary first-level indicators and 41 second-level indexes were selected (as shown in Table 3).

Table 3.A Pool of Evaluation Entries of State of Consciousness of Nonhuman Primates.
Items Score
Auditory
Localization to Sound 2
Auditory Startle 1
None 0
Visual
Normal 5
Object localization: Reaching 4
Visual Pursuit 3
Fixation 2
Visual Startle 1
None 0
Eye-opening
Spontaneous eye-opening 3
Eyes open to speech 2
Eyes open to pain 1
No response 0
Motor
Normal 6
Automatic Motor Response 5
Localization to Noxious Stimulation 4
Flexion/withdrawal to pain 3
Abnormal flexion to pain 2
Extension to pain 1
None 0
Orofacial movements
Normal 4
Vocalize 3
Moan 2
Oral Reflexive Movement 1
None 0
Pupil response
Normal 4
Sluggish 3
Unequal response 2
Unequal size 1
None 0
Brainstem reflexes
All present 4
Lash absent 3
Corneal absent 2
Doll’s eye/calorics absent 1
Carinal (all) absent 0
Breathing
Normal 4
Periodic 3
Central hyperventilation 2
Irregular hypoventilation 1
None (apnea) 0
3.1.3 Develop Expert Correspondence Questionnaire

To obtain expert opinions on the preliminary evaluation indicators, an expert correspondence questionnaire was developed. The expert correspondence questionnaire consisted of three parts:

(1) Letter to experts: Including background of the study, purpose, methodology, questionnaire completion, timing and contact information.

(2) Evaluation index judgment table at all levels: including index item index importance score and modification suggestions. A Likert five level scoring method was used to evaluate the importance of indicators and 1–5 points were counted from completely unimportant to very important. Experts assigned the degree of importance of indicators at all levels and put forward suggestions for modification, deletion, or addition.

(3) Basic information about experts was obtained, including age, education, professional title and other general information, as well as familiarity with the survey content and the judgment basis of the self-evaluation table.

3.1.4 Expert Selection and Consultation

According to the purpose of the study, experts who are familiar with the operation process of the clinical consciousness level behavior assessment scale or intimately familiar with research into the neuroscience of nonhuman primates were selected. Specific selection criteria were as follows:

(1) Experts had a deputy senior professional title or above.

(2) More than 10 years of clinical practice in consciousness disorders or research in nonhuman primate neuroscience.

(3) Bachelor degree or above.

(4) Willing to answer the expert consultation questionnaire in every rounds.

Questionnaires were emailed to experts and they were told to return them within two weeks. If the questionnaire was not clear or items were missing, they were asked to please confirm by phone. After the first round of questionnaires collected, statistical analysis was conducted on index scores, expert opinions were collated, the discussion was organized among research group members according to index screening standards and expert opinions and indicators then were adjusted; The evaluation results of the first round were fed back when the second round of questionnaires was issued. After two rounds of letter consultation, expert opinion on the evaluation of each indicator tended to be consistent and letter consultation was stopped and finally, the evaluation index system of nonhuman primate consciousness disorder was formed.

3.1.5 Statistical Analysis

The results of the questionnaire were checked and entered in Excel 2021(Microsoft Corp., Redmond, WA, USA) and SPSS 21.0 (IBM Corp., Chicago, IL, USA) by two researchers. The count data were described by frequency and percentage and the measurement data expressed by mean and standard deviation. The positive coefficient of experts is expressed by the recovery rate of the questionnaire and the concentration of expert opinion is expressed by the mean of importance value, standard deviation and full score rate. The authority coefficient (represented by Cr) was expressed by the arithmetic mean of the judgment coefficient (Ca) and the familiarity coefficient (Cs) and the calculation formula was Cr = (Ca + Cs)/2. The degree of coordination of expert opinions was expressed by the coefficient of variation (CV) and Kendall’s coefficient of concordance (Kendall’s W). The CV reflects the degree of dispersion of expert opinion, Kendall’s W valuates the degree of coordination among experts on the evaluation object. Its value is between 0 and 1 and the higher the value, representing the consistency of expert opinion, the better.

3.2 Validation of Evaluation Indexes

Additionally, to the clinical behavior assessment scale, which can be used to describe the level of consciousness of patients, the level of consciousness can also be assessed by objective techniques beyond consciousness. Currently recognized useful technologies are: EEG and neuroimaging (such as PET and fMRI). Bareham CA [37] used canonical correlation analysis to relate clinical (including CRS-R scores combined with demographic variables) and EEG variables to each other. This analysis revealed that the patient’s age, and the EEG theta band power and alpha band connectivity, all of those contributed most significantly to the relationship between EEG and clinical variables. Moreover, they found that EEG measures recorded at the time of assessment augmented clinical measures in predicting CRS-R scores at the next assessment. The bedside EEG assessments conducted at specialist nursing homes are certified to be feasible, and it has clinical utility and complete clinical knowledge and systematic behavioural assessments to inform prognosis and care. Stender J [38] did repeated standardised clinical assessments with the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R), cerebral (18)F-fluorodeoxy glucose (FDG) PET, and fMRI during mental activation tasks. The results suggest that cerebral (18)F-FDG PET could be used to complement bedside examinations and predict long-term recovery of patients with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome. Active fMRI could be useful for differential diagnosis. Both Ishizawa et al. [39] and Ballesteros et al. [40] used EEG to record neuronal dynamics in nonhuman primates during anesthesia induction from consciousness to loss of consciousness in real time, the latter also recorded the EEG signals during the recovery of consciousness in real time and the results showed that specific changes in EEG signals occurred during the recovery. Tasserie et al. [30] applied anesthesia to suppress consciousness in nonhuman primates. They found that during anesthesia, central thalamic stimulation induced arousal in an on-off manner and increased functional magnetic resonance imaging activity in prefrontal, parietal and cingulate cortices. These studies suggest that changes in the state of consciousness in nonhuman primates can be assessed using EEG and fMRI.

In the verification stage, consciousness will be assessed by the behavior assessment scale developed for the nonhuman primate model of consciousness disorder and animal behavior analysis software (such as DeepLabCut [41]). Other animal behavior analysis tools will be used to quantify behavior to verify each indicator. Meanwhile, objective techniques such as EEG and neuroimaging (such as PET and fMRI) will be applied to evaluate the level of consciousness in nonhuman primate animals models, compared with the results of the developed behavior assessment scale. Based on these information, the reliability and validity of scale and other measurement indicators can be tested. Additionally, some are clinically using extensive assessment scales or previously developed nonhuman primate consciousness assessment tools can also be used to assess the level of consciousness of nonhuman primates and observe which indices are related to EEG /fMRI/PET. Finally, results were compared with the Delphi method to verify whether the developed scale was meaningful for nonhuman primate consciousness assessment.

4. Conclusions

Loss of consciousness is a good model to study consciousness, including sleeping, anesthesia, coma and other disorders of consciousness. Currently, there are few studies on the assessment of consciousness in nonhuman primates. Tasserie [30] and Redinbaugh [31] proposed to evaluate the consciousness level of nonhuman primates in the process of anesthesia awakening based on clinical scales, but their evaluation method has not been confirmed to be accurate. Here, the interest is in loss of consciousness caused by disorders of consciousness such as coma. Although the hypothesis given above is also compiled based on clinical scales, the evaluation indicators proposed can be systematically verified by the Delphi method and associated objective techniques. A successful development of a behavioral scale for assessing the level of consciousness in nonhuman primates would be widely accepted in neuroscience. Meanwhile, it could overcome the difficulty of identifying the level of consciousness in the process of consciousness modeling of nonhuman primates and provide a valuable development to the field of consciousness and consciousness disorders.

5. Limitation and Outlook

The hypothesis described here is that nonhuman primate consciousness assessment scales can be constructed through literature review and Delphi method, with the main process being to extract indicators suitable for nonhuman primate consciousness assessment from existing consciousness assessment tools. Of course, it is not sufficient to construct a nonhuman primate consciousness scale by only a literature review, the selection of some indicators and evaluation of the importance of each indicator by experts. However, if behavioral changes of nonhuman primates can be observed in the process of consciousness recovery in combination with specific experiments, so as to explore the corresponding behavioral characteristics of different states of consciousness, a qualitative leap to the consciousness assessment scale constructed by Delphi method may be achieved.

Currently, it is difficult to construct a universally accepted scale for assessing the level of consciousness of nonhuman primates. Nonhuman primates are so valuable that studies are often limited by small sample sizes. And under the impact of the epidemic, it is difficult to carry out this work. So we want to use this hypothesis to call on a group of researchers who are interested in this research to do this work together. If a scientifically operable scale for assessing consciousness in nonhuman primates can be successfully developed, it may come into wide use and remove a further barrier to consciousness research.

Abbreviations

DoC, disorders of consciousness; RoC, recovery of consciousness; EEG, electroencephalography; PET, positron emission tomography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; PET-CT, positron emission tomography-computed tomography; SEP, somatosensory evoked potential; AEP, auditory evoked potentials; ERP, event-related potential; LoC, Loss of consciousness.

Author Contributions

WMS and CLM designed the study. CL performed the part research of nonhuman primates. XLD and CHL performed the part research of rehabilitation medicine. WMS and GXL wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to editorial changes in the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate

Not applicable.

Acknowledgment

We would like to express our gratitude to all those who helped us during the writing of this manuscript.

Funding

The work was supported in part by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31760276 and 31960171), the Jiangxi Natural Science Foundation (20171BAB204019 and 20192ACB20022), and the Jiangxi Provincial Special Fund for Postgraduate Innovation (YC2021-B021).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note: IMR Press stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References
[1]
Kent L, Van Doorn G, Klein B. Systema Temporis: a time-based dimensional framework for consciousness and cognition. Consciousness and Cognition. 2019; 73: 102766.
[2]
Benarroch EE. What is the Role of the Claustrum in Cortical Function and Neurologic Disease? Neurology. 2021; 96: 110–113.
[3]
Tononi G, Koch C. Consciousness: here, there and everywhere? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2015; 370: 20140167.
[4]
Pitts MA, Lutsyshyna LA, Hillyard SA. The relationship between attention and consciousness: an expanded taxonomy and implications for ‘no-report’ paradigms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2018; 373: 20170348.
[5]
Bayne T, Hohwy J, Owen AM. Are there Levels of Consciousness? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2016; 20: 405–413.
[6]
Fazekas P, Overgaard M. Multidimensional Models of Degrees and Levels of Consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2016; 20: 715–716.
[7]
Low P. Consciousness in human and non-human animals. In Panksepp J, et al. (eds.) The Francis Crick Memorial Conference: Cambridge, UK. 2012.
[8]
Ben-Haim MS, Dal Monte O, Fagan NA, et al. Disentangling perceptual awareness from nonconscious processing in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2021; 118: e2017543118.
[9]
Chang L, Zhang S, Poo M, Gong N. Spontaneous expression of mirror self-recognition in monkeys after learning precise visual-proprioceptive association for mirror images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2017; 114: 3258–3263.
[10]
Lyn H, Greenfield PM, Savage-Rumbaugh S, Gillespie-Lynch K, Hopkins WD. Nonhuman primates do declare! A comparison of declarative symbol and gesture use in two children, two bonobos, and a chimpanzee. Language and Communication. 2011; 31: 63–74.
[11]
Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND. Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. The Lancet Neurology. 2004; 3: 537–546.
[12]
Kang J, Huang L, Tang Y, Chen G, Ye W, Wang J, et al. A dynamic model to predict long-term outcomes in patients with prolonged disorders of consciousness. Aging. 2022; 14: 789–799.
[13]
Toker D, Pappas I, Lendner JD, Frohlich J, Mateos DM, Muthukumaraswamy S, et al. Consciousness is supported by near-critical slow cortical electrodynamics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2022; 119: e2024455119.
[14]
Giacino JT, Katz DI, Schiff ND, Whyte J, Ashman EJ, Ashwal S, et al. Practice guideline update recommendations summary: Disorders of consciousness: Report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination, and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology; the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine; and the National Institute on Disability, Independent Living, and Rehabilitation Research. Neurology. 2018; 91: 450–460.
[15]
Provencio JJ, Hemphill JC, Claassen J, Edlow BL, Helbok R, Vespa PM, et al. The Curing Coma Campaign: Framing Initial Scientific Challenges—Proceedings of the first Curing Coma Campaign Scientific Advisory Council Meeting. Neurocritical Care. 2020; 33: 1–12.
[16]
Schnakers C, Monti MM. Disorders of consciousness after severe brain injury: therapeutic options. Current Opinion in Neurology. 2017; 30: 573–579.
[17]
O’Donnell JC, Browne KD, Kilbaugh TJ, Chen HI, Whyte J, Cullen DK. Challenges and demand for modeling disorders of consciousness following traumatic brain injury. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2019; 98: 336–346.
[18]
Capitanio JP, Emborg ME. Contributions of non-human primates to neuroscience research. The Lancet. 2008; 371: 1126–1135.
[19]
Hofmeijer J, Beernink TMJ, Bosch FH, Beishuizen A, Tjepkema-Cloostermans MC, van Putten MJAM. Early EEG contributes to multimodal outcome prediction of postanoxic coma. Neurology. 2015; 85: 137–143.
[20]
Fan L, Li H, Zhuo J, Zhang Y, Wang J, Chen L, et al. The Human Brainnetome Atlas: a New Brain Atlas Based on Connectional Architecture. Cerebral Cortex. 2016; 26: 3508–3526.
[21]
Nuwer M. Assessment of digital EEG, quantitative EEG, and EEG brain mapping: report of the American Academy of Neurology and the American Clinical Neurophysiology Society. Neurology. 1997; 49: 277–292.
[22]
Riganello F, Vatrano M, Carozzo S, Russo M, Lucca LF, Ursino M, et al. The Timecourse of Electrophysiological Brain-Heart Interaction in DoC Patients. Brain sciences. 2021; 11: 750.
[23]
Pruvost-Robieux E, Marchi A, Martinelli I, Bouchereau E, Gavaret M. Evoked and Event-Related Potentials as Biomarkers of Consciousness State and Recovery. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2022; 39: 22–31.
[24]
Medina JP, Nigri A, Stanziano M, D’Incerti L, Sattin D, Ferraro S, et al. Resting-State fMRI in Chronic Patients with Disorders of Consciousness: The Role of Lower-Order Networks for Clinical Assessment. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12: 355.
[25]
Kujawa K, Zurek G, Kwiatkowska A, Olejniczak R, Żurek A. Assessment of Language Functions in Patients with Disorders of Consciousness Using an Alternative Communication Tool. Frontiers in Neurology. 2021; 12: 684362.
[26]
Zhang Y, Wang J, Schnakers C, He M, Luo H, Cheng L, et al. Validation of the Chinese version of the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R). Brain Injury. 2019; 33: 529–533.
[27]
Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2004; 85: 2020–2029.
[28]
Sherer M, Nakase-Thompson R, Yablon SA, Gontkovsky ST. Multidimensional Assessment of Acute Confusion after Traumatic Brain Injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2005; 86: 896–904.
[29]
Gennarelli TA, Thibault LE, Adams JH, Graham DI, Thompson CJ, Marcincin RP. Diffuse axonal injury and traumatic coma in the primate. Annals of Neurology. 1982; 12: 564–574.
[30]
Tasserie J, Uhrig L, Sitt J D, Manasova D, Dupont M, Dehaene S, et al. Deep brain stimulation of the thalamus restores signatures of consciousness in a nonhuman primate model. Science Advances. 2022; 8: eabl5547.
[31]
Redinbaugh MJ, Phillips JM, Kambi NA, Mohanta S, Andryk S, Dooley GL, et al. Thalamus Modulates Consciousness via Layer-Specific Control of Cortex. Neuron. 2020; 106: 66–75.e12.
[32]
Soriano JB, Murthy S, Marshall JC, Relan P, Diaz JV. A clinical case definition of post-COVID-19 condition by a Delphi consensus. The Lancet Infectious Diseases. 2022; 22: e102–e107.
[33]
Makhmutov R. The Delphi method at a glance. Pflege. 2021; 34: 221.
[34]
McPherson S, Reese C, Wendler MC. Methodology Update: Delphi Studies. Nursing research. 2018; 67: 404–410.
[35]
Romero-Collado A. Essential elements to elaborate a study with the (e)Delphi method. Elementos esenciales para elaborar un estudio con el método (e)Delphi. Enfermeria intensiva. 2021; 32: 100–104.
[36]
Diamond IR, Grant RC, Feldman BM, Pencharz PB, Ling SC, Moore AM, et al. Defining consensus: a systematic review recommends methodologic criteria for reporting of Delphi studies. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2014; 67: 401–409.
[37]
Bareham CA, Roberts N, Allanson J, Hutchinson PJA, Pickard JD, Menon DK, et al. Bedside EEG predicts longitudinal behavioural changes in disorders of consciousness. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2020; 28: 102372.
[38]
Stender J, Gosseries O, Bruno M, Charland-Verville V, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Demertzi A, et al. Diagnostic precision of PET imaging and functional MRI in disorders of consciousness: a clinical validation study. The Lancet. 2014; 384: 514–522.
[39]
Ishizawa Y, Ahmed OJ, Patel SR, Gale JT, Sierra-Mercado D, Brown EN, et al. Dynamics of Propofol-Induced Loss of Consciousness across Primate Neocortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016; 36: 7718–7726.
[40]
Ballesteros JJ, Briscoe JB, Ishizawa Y. Neural signatures of α2-Adrenergic agonist-induced unconsciousness and awakening by antagonist. eLife. 2020; 9: e57670.
[41]
Lauer J, Zhou M, Ye S, Menegas W, Schneider S, Nath T, et al. Multi-animal pose estimation, identification and tracking with DeepLabCut. Nature Methods. 2022; 19: 496–504.
Share
Back to top