IMR Press / JIN / Volume 18 / Issue 3 / DOI: 10.31083/j.jin.2019.03.183
Open Access Original Research
fNIRS correlates of the development of inhibitory control in young obese subjects
Show Less
1 Graduate School, Wuhan Sports University, Wuhan, 430079, P. R. China
2 Hubei Key Laboratory of Sport Training and Monitoring, College of Health Science, Wuhan Sports University, Wuhan, 430079, P. R. China
*Correspondence: 1990012@whsu.edu.cn (Mingsheng Xiong); sherry910527@hotmail.com (Jiaai Huang)
These authors contributed equally.
J. Integr. Neurosci. 2019, 18(3), 253–259; https://doi.org/10.31083/j.jin.2019.03.183
Submitted: 17 June 2019 | Accepted: 23 August 2019 | Published: 30 September 2019
Copyright: © 2019 Huang et al. Published by IMR press.
This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC 4.0 license https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/
Abstract

Executive functions are closely related to the prefrontal cortex, and inhibitory control is an important component of executive functioning. Previous studies have found that inhibitory control continues to develop after adolescence and that obesity is associated with executive functions. However, few studies have addressed whether obesity affects the development of inhibitory control. Hence, we focused on whether inhibitory control continues to develop after adolescence in obese individuals. We used a Stroop task to measure the inhibitory control of young obese subjects, and monitored accompanying brain activation by functional near-infrared spectroscopy technology. The findings suggest that brain activation due to Stroop interference does not increase with age in obese subjects and that early prevention of executive function deficit is recommended.

Keywords
Obesity
inhibitory control
development
fNIRS
Stroop color-word task
frontal cortical function
1. Introduction

Executive functions (also known as executive control or cognitive control) are top-down psychological processes required by individuals during attention or inhibiting irrational instinctual behavior (Diamond, 2013). Executive functions are closely related to academic achievement, social functioning, and emotional control in the development of individuals (Best et al., 2009; Liang et al., 2014). Inhibitory control is one of the most important components of executive functions. Inhibitory control usually refers to the ability to suppress automatic or dominant responses, but inhibition also requires interference control, directed forgetting, emotional control, and behavioral control (Nigg, 2000). Also, inhibitory control plays an important role in the development of individuals and can predict individual growth; for example, children with good inhibitory control are mentally and physically healthier in adulthood (Moffitt et al., 2011).

The development of inhibitory control continues after puberty. Previous behavioral studies have shown that the Stroop effect decreases when adolescence begins (Ikeda et al., 2011; Ordaz et al., 2013; Prencipe et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2011). Previous brain imaging studies have also reached relatively consistent conclusions. A study used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques and the Stroop paradigm to examine 30 participants aged 7 to 22 years and found that activation in the frontal lobe increased with age; activation was significantly higher in young adults than in adolescents, who, in turn, showed significantly greater activation than children (Adleman et al., 2002). Another study used functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) technology and the Stroop paradigm in 23 children and 14 adults to address this topic and revealed that brain activation (especially in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) caused by the Stroop effect increased with age from 7 to 29 years (Schroeter et al., 2004).

Executive function deficits are closely related to the prefrontal cortex. Clinical cases have shown that patients with prefrontal cortex lesions have difficulties with goal setting, planning, inhibition, and self-maintenance. Previous neuropsychological research has shown that the maturity of the frontal cortex parallels the development of executive functions (Garon et al., 2008; Luna et al., 2010; Ordaz et al., 2013; Prencipe et al., 2011; Shing et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2011). In the 1960s and 1970s, studies on human brain anatomy demonstrated that some brain regions, especially the prefrontal cortex, developed beyond childhood, and in the 1970s to 1980s, studies suggested that prefrontal cortex structure changed obviously in adolescence (Blakemore and Choudhury, 2006). In tasks that require the function of the frontal lobe, the performance of individuals followed a multi-polar developmental track, meaning that each subcomponent of the executive functions developed to maturity at a different time and in a different way. The most rapid period of development was from 6 to 8 years of age, during which abilities as concept formation, set-shifting, and rudimentary planning skills were present. Then, development tended be more gradual from late childhood and throughout adolescence continuing to develop into early adulthood (Ordaz et al., 2013; Tanaka et al., 2012). Previous studies have almost universally reached the consistent conclusion that executive functioning and the prefrontal cortex continue to develop after adolescence (Brown et al., 2005; Buttelmann and Karbach, 2017; Hsu et al., 2014; Luna et al., 2010; Prencipe et al., 2011; Satterthwaite et al., 2013; Shing et al., 2010).

In addition to age, obesity is another factor that influences the development of executive function. Previous studies have shown that obese children (or adolescents) exhibit lower inhibitory control than overweight children (or adolescents) (Pauli-Pott et al., 2010). The decline of cognitive function in children (or adolescents) is closely related to overweight (Li et al., 2008). Previous behavioral studies have shown that obese children have worse performance on the classical experimental task that is used to measure inhibitory control (Bruce et al., 2011; Wirt et al., 2014). A large number of executive function measurements have shown that obese children have significantly less control than healthy children, and obese adolescents have significant deficits in the structure of the prefrontal cortex (Liang et al., 2014). Brain imaging studies have also come to the same conclusion. For example, one study used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and the Stroop paradigm to compare 54 obese participants and 37 healthy participants, and the results showed that the body mass index (BMI) was negatively correlated with Stroop interference and gray matter volume (Maayan et al., 2011). However, few imaging studies have addressed whether obesity damages the development of inhibitory control; hence, we used the Stroop task and fNIRS to explore the relationship between age and inhibitory control in obese individuals.

Inhibitory control is important for the development of individuals, and age and obesity are two factors that influence the development of inhibitory control. Inhibitory control and the prefrontal cortex, whose functioning is related to being overweight, develop to maturity after adolescence. The relationship between executive function and obesity may be bidirectional, that is to say, executive function deficits may be a potential factor in individual BMI increases, while weight loss is helpful to a certain extent in executive function improvement of obese individuals (Xianlin et al., 2015).

To study the inhibitory control of obesity, we conducted experiments using fNIRS and the Stroop color-word task, which is the classic psychological task for measuring frontal cortex function (Adleman et al., 2002; Yennu et al., 2016). In the Stroop task, participants are instructed to name the color of font in which a word is printed and ignore the dominant tendency to read the meaning of the word (naming the color of the word is more difficult because it is not the habitual response); therefore, the task requires inhibitory control (Aron, 2010). Stroop interference is the difference between the congruent condition (in which the text color matches the meaning) and the incongruent condition (in which the word for one color is printed in another color) in the Stroop task. The greater the differences in reaction time and accuracy, the larger the Stroop effect was; the worse the inhibitory control ability, the lower the prefrontal cortex activation (Schroeter et al., 2007).

Brain imaging techniques like fNIRS use near-infrared light changes in living tissues to detect specific changes in the concentration of oxygen in the targeted brain regions. The advantage of fNIRS is that this technology is not very sensitive to head movement, and optical imaging is both readily accepted by participants and harmless to the human body (Timinkul et al., 2008). It also provides appropriate spatial and temporal resolution and is easily accepted even by children and obese participants.

2. Material and methods

To explore whether obesity would affect the development of inhibitory control, we used fNIRS to test 54 individuals while they performed a Stroop task. As BMI is negatively correlated with inhibitory control and its related brain region (the prefrontal cortex), we hypothesized that Stroop interference on behavioral tests would not decrease with age and that the accompanying frontal cortex brain activation would not increase with age in obese individuals.

2.1 Subjects

We enlisted 45 obese subjects with high body mass index (mean BMI in kg/m2 = 31.39, SD = 4.50) recruited from a fitness and weight loss summer camp participated in our study. All of them or their legal guardians signed informed consent forms. For subjects aged 18 - 25, based on the BMI cut-off points for obesity in the Chinese population, overweight (24 < BMI < 28 kg/m2) and obesity (BMI > 28 kg/m2) were defined. For subjects aged < 18, overweight and obesity were defined according to the 2004 Group of China Obesity Task Force definition (WGOC, 2004). Seven of them were excluded from the analyses because they could not complete the whole study or moved excessively during the study. Ultimately, 38 obese participants (24 male, 14 female) aged 9 to 25 old (mean age in years 16.11, SD = 4.75; 22 subjects aged < 18, 16 subjects aged 18 - 25) remained in this study (mean BMI = 31.55 kg/m2, SD = 4.17, range = 25.20 - 40.71 kg/m2, median = 31.62 kg/m2). Word reading plays an important role in the performance of the Stroop task; therefore, we chose subjects over the age of 8 years to ensure that reading performance would not be a confounding variable. The presence of psychological impairments was assessed using the Chinese version of Symptom Checklist-90 (SCL-90). All subjects showed standard scores compared to the national norm. Cognitive development was assessed using the Chinese version of WISC-III or WAIS-III. All participants scored higher than 80 for full-scale IQ. None of the subjects reported learning disabilities or abnormal neurological development, neither a history of developmental or psychological disorders. They were all right-handed and had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision and normal color vision. This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Wuhan Sports University.

2.2 Stimuli and procedures

We used E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA) to present the stimuli and to record the data. This study used a modified Stroop task for visual-cognitive stimulation to evoke event-related potentials (Ehlis et al., 2005). The stimuli consisted of four words: “red”, “green”, “blue” and “yellow”, and each word was printed in red, green, blue or yellow to produce interference between color words and their text colors. The congruent condition meant that the color of the word was congruent with the meaning of the word (e.g., the word “RED” was printed in red text); the incongruent condition meant that the color of the word was incongruent with the meaning of the word, e.g., the word “RED” was printed in blue text (see Fig. 1A). Stroop interference was measured as the difference between the incongruent and congruent conditions (the incongruent minus the congruent) (Chen et al., 2013; Xu et al., 2007). The experimental procedure is displayed in Fig. 1A. In each trial, a cross-shaped fixation point “+” was displayed for 1000 ms first, and then the stimulus was presented for 1000 ms, followed by a blank screen presented for 10000 ms. The participant was asked to press the button to indicate whether the meaning of the word was congruent with its printing color as quickly and accurately as possible. The order of the stimuli was random. The formal experiment consisted of 48 trials (24 congruent conditions and 24 incongruent conditions). Also, before the experiment, there was a practice session consisting of six trials to ensure that the participant understood the procedure.

Figure 1.

A. The design of the experimental runs and instance for the two conditions (congruent and incongruent conditions) of the modified Stroop task. Participants were instructed to decide whether the semantic meaning of the word corresponded to the color of the word. B. The fNIRS probe consisted of 8 dual-wavelength sources (●) and 7 optical detectors (○), and the numbers (1 to 20) indicate the fNIRS channels. Optical sources and detectors probe on the international 10-20 standard positions.

2.3 Data recording and analysis

We employed a multichannel continuous-wave fNIRS instrument (NIRX Medical Technologies LLC, USA). The fNIRS probe consisted of eight dual-wavelength sources (760 and 850 nm) and seven optical detectors. These probes covered the frontal cortex, and they were placed according to the 10-20 system, with some adjustments to make sure that each emitter was 3 cm from the corresponding detector (see Fig. 1B). Previous studies have demonstrated the positional relationship between the channels of fNIRS and specific brain regions (Okamoto et al., 2009; Tsuzuki et al., 2007). The sampling rate was set to 7.8 Hz. The hemoglobin data were band-pass filtered between 0.01 Hz and 0.3 Hz to remove baseline drift and physiological noise. Based on the modified Beer-Lambert law (MBLL), we acquired signals of both oxyhemoglobin (oxy-Hb) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (deoxy-Hb) in the monitored brain regions. As the oxy-Hb signal was more sensitive to the change of stimuli in the task, we analyzed only the oxy-Hb signal in the present study.

We used fNIRS to collect hemodynamic data from the congruent and incongruent conditions in all 20 channels during the Stroop task. Then, we analyzed the relationship between age and Stroop interference. Benjamini & Hochberg's method (Singh and Dan, 2006) was adopted for controlling the false discovery rate (FDR). Bayes factors (BF) were computed with Bayesian Pearson correlation for hypotheses testing, BF01 is expressed as odds for support or evidence for the H0 over H1. The behavioral and fNIRS results were analyzed with SPSS 25.0 software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results
3.1 Behavioral results

We observed significant differences between the congruent and incongruent conditions in both reaction time (RT) and accuracy (ACC). The mean RT in the incongruent condition (mean RT in ms = 1070.39, SD = 190.42) was significantly higher than in the congruent condition (mean RT in ms = 973.12, SD = 144.48). The mean ACC in the incongruent condition (mean ACC = 0.87, SD = 0.02) was significantly lower than in the congruent condition (mean ACC = 0.95, SD = 0.08). Paired-sample t-tests showed that there existed Stroop interference in both, RT (t(37) = 6.06, P < 0.01) and ACC (t(37) = 3.13, P < 0.01). As shown in Table 1, there was no significant difference in Stroop interference between male and female participants in RT (t(36) = 0.25, P = 0.81) or ACC (t(36) = 0.89, P = 0.38). Therefore, when we analyzed how age affected Stroop interference, we could ignore the factor of gender.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics and descriptive statistics (mean ± SD)
Male (n = 24) Female (n = 14) t
BMI (kg/m2) 32.41 ± 4.33 30.07 ± 3.56 1.71
Age 16.88 ± 4.92 14.79 ± 4.28 1.32
RT Interference 94.17 ± 116.69 102.56 ± 61.14 0.25
ACC Interference -0.10 ± 0.21 -0.05 ± 0.04 0.89

Note: Interference is the difference between the incongruent and congruent conditions in the Stroop task (the incongruent minus the congruent).

To test whether obesity would affect the development of inhibitory control, we assessed the relationship between Stroop interference and age. However, Pearson correlation coefficients showed no significant correlation between age and Stroop interference in RT (r = -0.28, P = 0.09) or in ACC (r = 0.19, P = 0.24). There was no significant correlation between BMI and Stroop interference in RT (r = -0.14, P = 0.41) or in ACC (r = 0.24, P = 0.14). A significant relationship between age and BMI (r = 0.50, P < 0.01) was detected, yet no significant partial correlation between age and Stroop interference in RT (r = -0.25, P = 0.14) or in ACC (r = 0.09, P = 0.62), after controlling for BMI. Bayesian analyses revealed anecdotal or moderate evidence for the null hypothesis (RT Interference BF01 =1.84, ACC Interference BF01 = 4.03).

3.2 fNIRS results

The hemodynamic Stroop interference effect was calculated as the difference of oxy-Hb between the incongruent and congruent conditions (the incongruent oxy-Hb minus the congruent oxy-Hb). Paired sample t-tests showed that at 8 channels a significant Stroop interference effect was captured (incongruent > congruent); these channels were ch3, ch4, ch7, ch11, ch15, ch16, ch19, and ch20 (q value of maximum FDR = 0.05, see Fig. 2). We converted all of the corresponding electrode positions to standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates using a probabilistic estimation method (Cutini et al., 2011; Szűcs et al., 2012). We determined the anatomical labels according to the MNI coordinates of all the corresponding electrodes and observed that the activated channels were located above the right frontopolar area (ch16), the bilateral orbitofrontal area (ch4 and ch11 for left; ch19 for right), the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (ch7 for left and ch15 for right) and the bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (ch3 for the left and 20 for the right). We calculated the Pearson correlation coefficient to investigate the relationship between the hemodynamic Stroop interference and BMI, but no significant correlation was found (P > 0.05). Also, there was no significant difference in hemodynamic Stroop interference between male and female participants (P > 0.05). There was no significant correlation between RT/ACC interference and hemodynamic Stroop interference (P > 0.05).

Figure 2.

The figure is a t-map of oxy-Hb signal change, and it reflects the Stroop interference effect (incongruent condition minus congruent condition). The color bar showed t-values, and the color is closer to red, the Stroop interference is more likely to be significant. We observed significant Stroop interference effects in the right frontopolar area, bilateral orbitofrontal area, bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Next, we investigate whether age was associated with hemodynamic Stroop interference. Hemodynamic Stroop interference was not significantly negatively associated with age in these eight channels (r range = -0.36 ~ -0.15, q value of maximum FDR = 0.05, see Fig. 3). There was no significant partial correlation between age and hemodynamic Stroop interference (r range = -0.34 ~ -0.11, q value of maximum FDR = 0.05), after controlling for BMI. Bayesian analyses revealed anecdotal or moderate evidence for the null hypothesis (BF01 range = 0.71 ~ 4.97). We next divided 20 of the 38 participants into two groups according to their age. The youngest ten participants represented the bottom 27% of their age (BG: mean age in years 10.70, SD = 1.06; mean BMI = 29.35 kg/m2, SD = 3.40) and other ten participants represented the upper 27% of their age (UG: mean age in years 22.40, SD = 1.77; mean BMI = 33.01 kg/m2, SD = 4.73). There was no significant age effect on RT interference (t(18) = 1.71, P = 0.11), ACC interference (t(18) = -0.96, P = 0.35), and hemodynamic interference (q value of maximum FDR = 0.05) between the BG and the UG.

Figure 3.

Oxy-Hb interference due to Stroop interference (y-axis) across age (x-axis) in ch3, ch4, ch7, ch11, ch15, ch16, ch19, and ch20. Pearson correlation coefficients showed no significantly negative correlation between age and hemodynamic Stroop interference (r range = -0.36 ~ -0.15, q value of maximum FDR = 0.05).

4. Discussion

Our study suggested that the behavioral performance and prefrontal cortex activation of 38 obese participants in the Stroop task did not increase with age. We found that RT was significantly higher in the incongruent condition than in the congruent condition and that ACC was significantly lower in the incongruent condition than in the congruent condition. Thus, our study successfully elicited Stroop interference following previous literature (Ehlis et al., 2005). Then, we found that age was not associated with Stroop interference in RT or ACC. In other words, behavioral Stroop interference did not decrease throughout cognitive development. This result differed from those of previous research (Huizinga et al., 2006). Also, we found that prefrontal cortex activation did not increase with age. The neural result was also inconsistent with previous studies in normal-weight participants, which showed that inhibitory control developed after adolescence. For example, an fMRI study found a positive correlation between ages from 7 to 22 years and Stroop-related activation in the left lateral prefrontal cortex in normal-weight participants. They also found that Stroop-task-related functional development of the prefrontal cortex continues to develop into adulthood (Adleman et al., 2002).

Another fNIRS study (Schroeter et al., 2004) found that the brain activation due to Stroop interference significantly increased with age (from 7 to 29 years) in the DLPFC in normal-weight participants. In the present study, activation during the Stroop task did not increase with age from childhood to young adulthood, implying that the neural mechanisms used to suppress the Stroop effect did not develop in obese individuals as it does in normal-weight people. This inconsistency might be explained by the fact that we included obese individuals as our participants, unlike Schroeter et al. (2004) or others did, since obesity might affect the association between age and the Stroop interference-effect. Although not consistent with the current literature about the non-obese population, the findings of obese participants in our study can be regarded as a novel, but a meaningful phenomenon that warrants further study. As a review points out, the heavy burden of overweight and obesity during childhood and adolescence and its effects on cognitive control development (Esteban-Cornejo et al., 2018).

Executive function deficits are closely related to the prefrontal cortex. Clinical cases have shown that patients with prefrontal cortex lesions have difficulties with goal setting, planning, inhibition and self-maintenance, and previous neuropsychological research has shown that the maturity of the frontal cortex parallels the development of the executive functions (Leon-Carrion et al., 2004). Studies using non-invasive brain stimulation procedures have found that the DLPFC implicated in inhibitory control. One study found that repeated tDCS for 5 days over the right DLPFC may induce a more persistent decrease in self-reported craving, probably because enhancing the right DLPFC activity may strengthen inhibitory control (Ljubisavljevic et al., 2016).

Overweight or obese individuals often show deficits in executive function compared to normal-weight individuals. In childhood, obese individuals have more difficulty in response inhibition than lean individuals (Nederkoorn et al., 2007). A systematic literature review showed that obese adolescent individuals (from 13 to 18 years of age) had poorer performance than healthy individuals in tasks that depend on inhibitory control and, furthermore, had smaller OFC volume (Reinert et al., 2013). In an adult population, Willeumier et al. (2011) found that high BMI might be a risk factor for decreased prefrontal cortex function and potentially impaired executive function. Our study found that in obese subjects, brain activation during the Stroop task did not increase with age and decreased in the right DLPFC and the OFC, implying that obesity affected the development of those two areas.

Obesity itself has a compounding negative impact on the brain via mechanisms currently attributed to low-grade systemic inflammation, elevated lipids, and/or insulin resistance (Smith et al., 2011). Components of the immune system involved in inflammation can alter neural, cognitive, and motivational processes that lead to impaired self-regulation and poor health (Shields et al., 2017). Therefore, we recommend early prevention of impaired executive function. A previous study showed that the performance of participants on a Stroop task improved after dieting (Makris et al., 2013). Also, aerobic exercise helped improve executive functions (Kamijo et al., 2012). Furthermore, regular exercise can affect brain plasticity and increase the volumes of gray matter and white matter in the brain (Dupuy et al., 2015). Therefore, exercising to lose weight might be a good way for young people to improve their executive function ability and promote the healthy development of the prefrontal cortex.

We did not find a correlation between BMI and brain activation during the Stroop task. One possible explanation is that, although BMI is commonly used to measure obesity, it is not a very accurate predictor of cognitive processes and does not represent body fat percentage very well (Nuttall, 2015). If we could find a better index of obesity that predicted cognitive processes more accurately, then we could examine the relationship between obesity and various health risks through functional neuroimaging. For example, waist circumference is a more sensitive indicator of obesity, particularly in females, for potentially determining the adverse effects of obesity and overweight on the brain and associated risks to health (Kurth et al., 2013). Another reason might be the design of the present study procedure; the difficulty of the Stroop task was lowered to adapt the test to child participants. Research on developmental inhibitory control may be affected by the flexibility of the task, and complex cognitive tasks can reflect those small changes (Buttelmann and Karbach, 2017).

Inhibitory control plays a very important developmental role but obesity impairs its development. In healthy individuals, inhibitory control continues to develop into adulthood. However, in obese individuals, inhibitory control does not follow the same developmental trajectory, possibly because obesity impairs prefrontal cortex function (Willeumier et al., 2011). Therefore, we recommend early prevention of impaired executive function.

Acknowledgment

We thank all the participants for their participation. This research was supported by the Science Fund for Hubei Superior Discipline Groups of Physical Education and Health Promotion, National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 81971661), the project from General Administration of Sport of China (Grant No. 2014B094), and Natural Science Foundation of Hubei Province (Grant No. 2016CFA098).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

References
[1]
Adleman, N. E., Menon, V., Blasey, C. M., White, C. D., Warsofsky, I. S., Glover, G. H. and Reiss, A. L. (2002) A developmental fMRI study of the Stroop color-word task. NeuroImage 16, 61-75.
[2]
Aron, A. R. (2010) Progress in executive-function research: from tasks to functions to regions to networks. Current Directions in Psychological Science 17, 124-129.
[3]
Best, J. R., Miller, P. H. and Jones, L. L. (2009) Executive functions after age 5: changes and correlates. Developmental Review 29, 180-200.
[4]
Blakemore, S. J. and Choudhury, S. (2006) Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 47, 296-312.
[5]
Brown, T. T., Lugar, H. M., Coalson, R. S., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, S. E., and Schlaggar, B. L. (2005) Developmental changes in human cerebral functional organization for word generation. Cerebral Cortex 15, 275-290.
[6]
Bruce, A. S., Black, W. R., Bruce, J. M., Daldalian, M., Martin, L. E. and Davis, A. M. (2011) Ability to delay gr.pngication and BMI in preadolescence. Obesity 19, 1101-1102.
[7]
Buttelmann, F. and Karbach, J. (2017) Development and plasticity of cognitive flexibility in early and middle childhood. Frontiers in Psychology 8, 1040-1040.
[8]
Chen, A., Tang, D. and Chen, X. (2013) Training reveals the sources of Stroop and Flanker interference effects. PLoS One 8, e76580.
[9]
Cutini, S., Scatturin, P. and Zorzi, M. (2011) A new method based on ICBM152 head surface for probe placement in multichannel fNIRS. NeuroImage 54, 919-927.
[10]
Diamond, A. (2013) Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64, 135-168.
[11]
Dupuy, O., Gauthier, C. J., Fraser, S. A., Desjardins-Crèpeau, L., Desjardins, M., Mekary, S., Lesage, F., Hoge, R. D., Pouliot, P. and Bherer, L. (2015) Higher levels of cardiovascular fitness are associated with better executive function and prefrontal oxygenation in younger and older women. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 66-66.
[12]
Ehlis, A. C., Herrmann, M. J., Wagener, A., and Fallgatter, A. J. (2005) Multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy detects specific inferior-frontal activation during incongruent Stroop trials. Biological Psychology 69, 315-331.
[13]
Esteban-Cornejo, I., Ortega, F. B. and Catena, A. (2018) Neural perspectives on cognitive control development during childhood and adolescence should take into account how obesity affects brain development. Acta Paediatrica 107, 720-721.
[14]
Force, G. C. O. T. (2004) Body mass index reference norm for screening overweight and obesity in chinese children and adolescents. Chinese Journal of Epidemiology 25, 97-102.
[15]
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E. and Smith, I. M. (2008) Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin 134, 31-60.
[16]
Hsu, N. S., Novick, J. M. and Jaeggi, S. M. (2014) The development and malleability of executive control abilities. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8, 221-221.
[17]
Huizinga, M., C. V. and van der Molen, M. W, . (2006) Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2017-2036.
[18]
Ikeda, Y., Okuzumi, H., Kokubun, M. and Haishi, K. (2011) Age-related trends of interference control in school-age children and young adults in the Stroop color-word test. Psychological Reports 108, 577-584.
[19]
Kamijo, K., Khan, N. A., Po.pngex, M. B., Scudder, M. R., Drollette, E. S., Raine, L. B., Evans, E. M., Castelli, D. M. and Hillman, C. H. (2012) The relation of adiposity to cognitive control and scholastic achievement in preadolescent children. Obesity 20, 2406-2411.
[20]
Kurth, F., Levitt, J. G., Phillips, O. R., Luders, E., Woods, R. P., Mazziotta, J. C., Toga, A. W. and Narr, K. L. (2013) Relationships between gray matter, body mass index, and waist circumference in healthy adults. Human Brain Mapping 34, 1737-1746.
[21]
Leon-Carrion, J., García-Orza, J. and Pérez-Santamaría, F. J. (2004) Development of the inhibitory component of the executive functions in children and adolescents. International Journal of Neuroscience 114, 1291-1311.
[22]
Li, Y., Dai, Q., Jackson, J. C. and Zhang, J. (2008) Overweight is associated with decreased cognitive functioning among school-age children and adolescents. Obesity 16, 1809-1815.
[23]
Liang, J., Matheson, B. E., Kaye, W. H. and Boutelle, K. N. (2014) Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. International Journal of Obesity 38, 494-506.
[24]
Ljubisavljevic, M., Maxood, K., Bjekic, J., Oommen, J. and Nagelkerke, N. (2016) Long-term effects of repeated prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tdcs) on food craving in normal and overweight young adults. Brain Stimulation 9, 826-833.
[25]
Luna, B., Padmanabhan, A. and O'Hearn, K. (2010) What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence? Brain and Cognition 72, 101-113.
[26]
Maayan, L., Hoogendoorn, C., Sweat, V. and Convit, A. (2011) Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity 19, 1382-1387.
[27]
Makris, A., Darcey, V. L., Rosenbaum, D. L., Komaroff, E., Vander Veur, S. S., Collins, B. N., Klein, S., Wyatt, H. R. and Foster, G. D. (2013) Similar effects on cognitive performance during high- and low-carbohydrate obesity treatment. Nutrition & Diabetes 3, e89-e89.
[28]
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., Houts, R., Poulton, R., Roberts, B. W., Ross, S., Sears, M. R., Thomson, W. M. and Caspi, A. (2011) A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108, 2693-2698.
[29]
Nederkoorn, C., Jansen, E., Mulkens, S. and Jansen, A. (2007) Impulsivity predicts treatment outcome in obese children. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45, 1071-1075.
[30]
Nigg, J. T. (2000) On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin 126, 220-246.
[31]
Nuttall, F. Q. (2015) Body mass index: obesity, bmi, and health: a critical review. Nutrition Today 50, 117-128.
[32]
Okamoto, M., Tsuzuki, D., Clowney, L., Dan, H., Singh, A. K. and Dan, I. (2009) Structural atlas-based spatial registration for functional near-infrared spectroscopy enabling inter-study data integration. Clinical Neurophysiology 120, 1320-1328.
[33]
Ordaz, S. J., Foran, W., Velanova, K. and Luna, B. (2013) Longitudinal growth curves of brain function underlying inhibitory control through adolescence. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 18109-18124.
[34]
Pauli-Pott, U., Albayrak, O., Hebebrand, J. and Pott, W. (2010) Association between inhibitory control capacity and body weight in overweight and obese children and adolescents: dependence on age and inhibitory control component. Child Neuropsychology 16, 592-603.
[35]
Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., and Zelazo, P. D. (2011) Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108, 621-637.
[36]
Reinert, K. R. S., Po'e, E. K. and Barkin, S. L. (2013) The relationship between executive function and obesity in children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. Journal of Obesity 2013, 820956-820956.
[37]
Satterthwaite, T. D., Wolf, D. H., Erus, G., Ruparel, K., Elliott, M. A., Gennatas, E. D., Hopson, R., Jackson, C., Prabhakaran, K., Bilker, W. B., Calkins, M. E., Loughead, J., Smith, A., Roalf, D. R., Hakonarson, H., Verma, R., Davatzikos, C., Gur, R. C. and Gur, R. E. (2013) Functional maturation of the executive system during adolescence. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 16249-16261.
[38]
Schroeter, M. L., Cutini, S., Wahl, M. M., Scheid, R. and Yves von Cramon, D. (2007) Neurovascular coupling is impaired in cerebral microangiopathy--an event-related Stroop study. Neuroimage 34, 26-34.
[39]
Schroeter, M. L., Zysset, S., Wahl, M. and von Cramon, D. Y. (2004) Prefrontal activation due to Stroop interference increases during development--an event-related fNIRS study. NeuroImage 23, 1317-1325.
[40]
Shields, G. S., Moons, W. G. and Slavich, G. M. (2017) Inflammation, self-regulation, and health: an immunologic model of self-regulatory failure. Perspectives on Psychological Science 12, 588-612.
[41]
Shing, Y. L., Lindenberger, U., Diamond, A., Li, S. C. and Davidson, M. C. (2010) Memory maintenance and inhibitory control differentiate from early childhood to adolescence. Developmental Neuropsychology 35, 679-697.
[42]
Singh, A. K. and Dan, I. (2006) Exploring the false discovery rate in multichannel NIRS. Neuroimage 33, 542-549.
[43]
Smith, E., Hay, P., Campbell, L. and Trollor, J. N. (2011) A review of the association between obesity and cognitive function across the lifespan: implications for novel approaches to prevention and treatment. Obesity Reviews 12, 740-755.
[44]
Szűcs, D., Killikelly, C. and Cutini, S. (2012) Event-related near-infrared spectroscopy detects conflict in the motor cortex in a Stroop task. Brain Research 1477, 27-36.
[45]
Tanaka, C., Matsui, M., Uematsu, A., Noguchi, K. and Miyawaki, T. (2012) Developmental trajectories of the fronto-temporal lobes from infancy to early adulthood in healthy individuals. Developmental Neuroscience 34, 477-487.
[46]
Timinkul, A., Kato, M., Omori, T., Deocaris, C. C., Ito, A., Kizuka, T., Sakairi, Y., Nishijima, T., Asada, T. and Soya, H. (2008) Enhancing effect of cerebral blood volume by mild exercise in healthy young men: a near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuroscience Research 61, 242-248.
[47]
Tsuzuki, D., Jurcak, V., Singh, A. K., Okamoto, M., Watanabe, E. and Dan, I. (2007) Virtual spatial registration of stand-alone fNIRS data to MNI space. NeuroImage 34, 1506-1518.
[48]
Willeumier, K. C., Taylor, D. V. and Amen, D. G. (2011) Elevated BMI is associated with decreased blood flow in the prefrontal cortex using SPECT imaging in healthy adults. Obesity 19, 1095-1097.
[49]
Wirt, T., V., Hundsdörfer, Schreiber, A., Kesztyüs, D., Steinacker, J. M. and “Komm mit in das gesunde Boot - Grundschule” Research. ( 2014) Associations between inhibitory control and body weight in German primary school children. Eating Behaviors 15, 9-12.
[50]
Wu, K. K., Chan, S. K., Leung, P. W. L., Liu, W. S., Leung, F. L. T. and Ng, R. (2011) Components and developmental differences of executive functioning for school-aged children. Developmental Neuropsychology 36, 319-337.
[51]
Xianlin, Y. I., Wang, M and Wang, X. ( 2015) The relationship between executive functions and pediatric obesity epidemic. Advances in Psychological Science 23, 1920-1920. 8314c17299fdcf37b1b2a5974e8d8bd8http%3A%2F%2Fwww.en.cnki.com.cn%2FArticle_en%2FCJFDTotal-XLXD201511006.htm
[52]
Xu, J., Mendrek, A., Cohen, M. S., Monterosso, J., Simon, S., Jarvik, M., Olmstead, R., Brody, A. L., Ernst, M. and London, E. D. (2007) Effect of cigarette smoking on prefrontal cortical function in nondeprived smokers performing the Stroop task. Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 1421-1428.
[53]
Yennu, A., Tian, F., Smith-Osborne, A., Gatchel, R. J., Woon, F. L. and Liu, H. (2016) Prefrontal responses to Stroop tasks in subjects with post-traumatic stress disorder assessed by functional near infrared spectroscopy. Scie.pngic Reports 6, 30157-30157.
Share
Back to top