IMR Press / JIN / Volume 17 / Issue 3 / DOI: 10.31083/JIN-170057
Open Access Brief Report
Microsaccadic behavior when developing a complex dynamical activity
Show Less
1 Instituto de Investigaciones en Ingeniería Eléctrica (UNS-CONICET), Bahía Blanca, Buenos Aires, Argentina
2 Universidad Nacional del Sur (UNS), Bahía Blanca, Argentina
3 Instituto de Investigaciones Bioquímicas de Bahía Blanca (INIBIBB) (UNS-CONICET), Bahía Blanca, Buenos Aires, Argentina
*Correspondence: gerardo.fernandez@uns.edu.ar (Gerardo Fernández)
J. Integr. Neurosci. 2018, 17(3), 287–290; https://doi.org/10.31083/JIN-170057
Submitted: 22 June 2017 | Accepted: 17 October 2017 | Published: 15 August 2018
Abstract

Microsaccades are sensitive to changes of perceptual inputs as well as modulations of cognitive states. There are just a few works analyzing microsaccades while subjects are processing complex information and fewer when subjects make predictions about upcoming events. To evaluate whether contextual predictability might change microsaccadic behavior, microsaccades were evaluated for twenty-one subjects when reading 40 regular sentences and 40 proverbs. Maxjump was defined as the word with the largest difference between the cloze predictability of two consecutive words. Analysis of microsaccades while reading proverbs and regular sentences revealed that microsaccadic rate on words before Maxjump, during Maxjump and words after Maxjump varied depending on the kind of sentence and on the word predictability. Words of low and high predictability required either less or more microsaccades to previous words, during and on Maxjump, depending upon the semantic context and a readers' predictions of upcoming words. The present study demonstrates that the rate of microsaccades showed significant differences for reading either proverbs or regular sentences. Hence, evaluation of microsaccades while reading sentences with different contextual predictability may provide information concerning specific effects of cue attention during a complex task.

Keywords
Microsaccade
reading
proverbs
attentional cue
predictions
1. Introduction

Reading, eye movements, and visual attention are closely related. Readers acquire visual information during brief periods of fixation that are separated by saccades. The major function of saccadic eye movement is to move words into the fovea for close inspection during the subsequent fixation [1]. Although it is common to use the term "fixation", an acute examination of motor activity between saccades reveals that the eyes are never immobile. During these periods, small eye movements occur that consist of the continual alternation between miniature saccades (i.e. microsaccade) and periods of relatively slow eye drifts [2]. It has been proposed that microsaccades provide a mechanism for relocating gaze in high acuity tasks [3]. Investigators have reported that microsaccade properties are sensitive to changes of perceptual inputs as well as modulation of cognitive states [4,5]. It has been shown that microsaccades follow spatial attention, suggesting that microsaccades are an expression of ongoing physiological processes, whose main functions might be the maintenance of gaze fixation [1].

Reading is useful for understanding how eye movements and cognitive functions act together. There have been large syntactic and semantic reading studies undertaken with the goal of understanding how word properties and contextual predictability affects eye movement [6,7]. Nevertheless, the effect of how subjects perform real-time word processing and how this impacts microsaccade modulation is still unknown. The analysis of microsaccades during the reading of sentences with different cloze predictability (i.e., regular sentences and proverbs) could provide new answers.

Recent works suggest that overt shifts of attention and eye movements are linked to visuospatial memory [8,9]. Also, known targets enhance the speed and accuracy with which stimuli are detected and discriminated [10,11]. Thus, visuospatial attention and oculomotor planning may reinforce and help to produce stronger visual representations. Investigators have proposed that expectations act as an attentional filter to facilitate the extraction of information. The prevailing view is that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) mediates this pre-stimulus activity modulation. Extensive evidence suggests that the PFC generates cortical processing in visual areas via cognitive (i.e. top-down) signals prior to stimulus presentation [12,13,14]. Expectation, or anticipation, that precedes the presentation of a stimulus and can be predicted is a type of top-down modulation that has been shown to involve the PFC [10]. Investigators suggest that, due to prediction, the PFC facilitates goal-directed saccades [15]. On the other hand, the superior colliculus (SC), the top-level brainstem structure involved in the control of saccadic eye movements [16,17], has been identified as a key neural structure for microsaccadic generation [18,19]. Recently, it was proposed that the PFC provides an excitatory input to the SC to facilitate task goals [15].

To date, there have been few studies that have explicitly investigated microsaccadic behavior during the performance of complex tasks and even fewer cases that have studied the prediction of upcoming events [20]. Analysis of sentences with different predictability provides an excellent tool for understanding microsaccadic behavior in complex top-down processes. The hypothesis here is that contextual predictability would probably change the microsaccade rate due to top-down attentional cues. To test this hypothesis, it was investigated whether the properties of a sentence affected microsaccade rate. To this end, proverbs and regular sentences were employed as reading material. When reading proverbs, there is typically a word at which not only the next word but the entire sentence becomes predictable. To capture this sharp transition in predictability that occurs when a subject matches an entire sentence being read to one held in memory, the word with the maximum change in cloze predictability was determined (see Methods section for more detailed description) relative to the previous word in a given sentence. In the context of reading, this word was referred to as the Maxjump word [7]. Consequently, the words in a sentence were divided into three groups, depending on their position in relation to the Maxjump word. In the first group of words were words before the Maxjump word, in the second group was the Maxjump word and in the last group were the words following the Maxjump word. Eyetracker was used to evaluate the effect of predictability of different groups of words on microsaccade rate.

2. Methods

Data were acquired from 21 readers (all native Spanish speakers), mean age 28 (SD = 4.2 years) and mean education 18.2 years.

Putative subjects diagnosed with ophthalmologic diseases such as glaucoma, visually significant cataracts, or macular degeneration, or having a visual acuity less than 20/20 were excluded from the study.

2.1 Apparatus and eye movement data

Single sentences were presented on the center line of a 20-inch LCD Monitor (1024 $\times$ 768 pixels resolution; font: regular; New Courier; 12 point, 0.2$ ^\circ $ in height). Subjects sat at a distance of 60 cm from the monitor. Head movements were minimized using a chin rest. An EyeLink 1000 Desktop Mount (SR Research) eyetracker, with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz, was used to record the eye movements.

Subject gaze was calibrated with a standard 13-point grid for both eyes. They were instructed to read single sentences in silence. A random question related to a given sentence occurred in about 20% of the trials. Subjects used a computer mouse to choose one of three alternative options to provide the correct answer.

The sentence corpus was composed of 40 regular sentences (e.g. "Ayer charle con Laura acerca de su hija" English translation, "Yesterday I talked to Laura about her daughter") and 40 proverbs (e.g. "Mejor pajaro en mano que cien volando" English translation, "A bird in the hand is worth two in the bush"). For full details of sentence and word properties, see Fernandez et al. [7].

The word with the largest difference between the cloze predictability of two consecutive words was determined according to the following equation:

$\text{jump word} = \max [\text{Logit(pred}_{N+1}) -\text{Logit(pred}_{N})].$

The jump-word separates the sentence in three regions: before, during, and after maxjump word. With this Maxjump variable the contextual word predictability effect due to memory retrieval was tested [7, 21].

The methodology employed to extract the microsaccade from each fixation, was based on Engbert and Kliegl [22].

3 Results

Word information is processed in the brain during fixations and during this process microsaccades are generated. It was hypothesized that context predictability would affect microsaccades. To investigate this hypothesis, changes in microsaccade rate were evaluated, when comparing proverbs and regular sentences. As shown in Fig. 1 the mean microsaccade rate for words previous to Maxjump, during Maxjump, and after Maxjump, varied depending upon both the kind of sentence and on word predictabilities. When analyzing words previous to Maxjump, words with either low predictability or words with high predictability exhibited well differentiated rates of microsaccade, which was also found to be the case for the analysis of proverbs and regular sentences. The microsaccade rate for words prior to Maxjump in proverbs were 10.38 sec vs. 10.65 sec and in regular sentences were 10.26 sec vs. 9.90 sec, for low predicability words and high predictability words, respectively. It seems that proverbs required a higher rate of microsaccade (See Fig. 1). The difference between sentence types when considering microsaccade rate was significant $ (p = 0.01) $. For the Maxjump words, the microsaccade rates for proverbs were 10.52 sec vs. 9.63 sec and in regular sentences, 10.26 sec vs. 9.63 sec, for low predictability words and high predictability words, respectively. In this case the rate of microsaccade was similar for both kinds of sentence and the effect of the sentence was not significant $ (p = 0.95) $. Finally, on words after Maxjump, the microsaccade rate for proverbs were 10.30 sec vs. 10.26 sec and in regular sentences were 9.98 sec vs. 10.38 sec, for low predictability words and high predictability words, respectively. The difference between sentence types when considering microsaccade rate was significant $ (p = 0.02) $. Note that in this case regular sentences showed a major rate of microsaccade on high predictable words similar to those reported in proverbs when processing words on the previous Maxjump (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

The fixations analyzed are divided in words with low and high predictability. For each group of words the microsaccade rate prior to maxjump, during maxjump and after maxjump were analyzed. Mean microsaccade rate corresponding to proverbs (red) and regular sentences (turqoise) are illustrated.

4. Discussion

Several functions have been proposed for microsaccades: Counteracting perceptual fading, improving gaze in high acuity tasks, and following spatial attention [1, 23]. Reading material provides an important tool for analyzing microsaccadic behavior. The present results reveal that the rate of microsaccades showed significant differences when reading proverbs and regular sentences.

Analysis of microsaccades showed different mean rates for words previous, during, and after maxjump, and between proverbs and regular sentences. The microsaccade rate were higher when processing proverbs, in particular on words prior to maxjump (Fig.1). This suggests the presence of a top-down microsaccade manipulation when processing proverbs, because a retrieval process was required for updating such words. When reading proverbs the predictability of words previous and after maxjump increased and decreased microsaccade rates, respectively. This particular effect suggests that microsaccade activity is present performing predictions about upcoming words, whereas in regular sentences these effects of word predictability seem to be the opposite, at least until after maxjump, where readers are able to form predictions of upcoming words [7] (see slopes Fig. 1). Increasing predictability in proverbs increased differences in word processing between both groups of sentences. Results imply that semantic context has an impact on microsaccadic rate, with microsaccades being actively involved during top-down processes for reading and anticipating words.

Recent reports suggest that the PFC contributes an excitatory signal that facilitates the generation of goal-directed saccades. As stated in the Introduction section, there is evidence that the PFC provides an excitatory input to the SC for facilitating behavioral goals [16]. The SC was identified as a key neural structure for microsaccade generation [18]. The prevailing view is that the PFC mediates this prestimulus activity modulation generating cortical processing in visual areas via top-down signals prior to stimulus presentation [24]. A type of top-down [13] modulation that has been shown to involve the PFC is expectation, or anticipation, which precedes the presentation of a stimulus that can be predicted [25]. Prediction is a fundamental aspect of visual perception. The present results show a clear relationship between microsaccade rate and prediction of upcoming words. Further, differentiated processing of microsaccade rate was needed for anticipating known words embedded in proverbs. Recently, the microsaccade of expert and novice ping pong players were analyzed where they had to predict pingpong ball trajectory [20]. Expert player feedback was associated with smaller values of microsaccade parameters. Such results are similar to those reported here, suggesting that better prediction modifies microsaccade rate.

It was recently suggested [26] that the inferior parietal lobe mediates the automatic allocation of attention to retrieved memory contents, while others [8] proposed that overt shifts of attention through eye movements are associated with higher accuracy of performance in relational visuospatial memory task [27,28,29]. It was argued that activation of these parietal lobe regions may reinforce and help to produce stronger relational spatial representations and, consequently, produce more accurate memory recognitions, while fewer bottom-up resources are used. This study showed that microsaccade rate can be influenced by cognitive cues in a specific task. Proverbs might provide an example of the specific effect of cue attention on ongoing word processing and microsaccade execution.

Acknowledgments

This work was partially supported by grants PGI 24/K062, Universidad Nacional del Sur and PICT 2013-0403, Agencia Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología, Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología, Argentina. We thank Silvana Sañudo and Marcela Schumacher for registering eye movements.

Conflict of Interest

All authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References
[1]
Laubrock J, Kliegl R, Rolfs M, Engbert R ( 2010) When do microsaccades follow spatial attention? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 72( 3), 683-694.
[2]
Poletti M, Rucci M ( 2016) A compact field guide to the study of microsaccades: Challenges and functions. Vision Research 118, 83-97. 10.1016/j.visres.2015.01.0182568931542e36f9556867590fe931207ee6108efhttp%3A%2F%2Feuropepmc.org%2Fabstract%2Fmed%2F25689315https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0042698915000358
[3]
Ko H-k, Poletti M, Rucci M ( 2010) Microsaccades precisely relocate gaze in a high visual acuity task. Nature Neuroscience 13( 12), 1549-1553. 10.1038/nn.2663210375838470b2a4ae0e547e1cd7d08083c3115bhttp%3A%2F%2Fwww.nature.com%2Fneuro%2Fjournal%2Fv13%2Fn12%2Fabs%2Fnn.2663.htmlhttp://www.nature.com/articles/nn.2663
[4]
Hafed ZM, Clark JJ ( 2002) Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts. Vision Research 42( 22), 2533-2545. 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00263-8124458477496eed91caa342b04ad585747cf3d9chttp%3A%2F%2Fwww.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov%2Fpubmed%2F12445847http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0042698902002638
[5]
Engbert R ( 2012) Computational modeling of collicular integration of perceptual responses and attention in microsaccades. Journal of Neuroscience 32( 23), 8035-8039. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0808-12.201222674278f62f56095e0b540d68d9ac4f851ec7afhttp%3A%2F%2Fwww.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov%2Fpubmed%2F22674278http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0808-12.2012
[6]
Rayner K ( 1998) Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124( 3), 372. 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372
[7]
Fernández G, Shalom DE, Kliegl R, Sigman M ( 2014) Eye movements during reading proverbs and regular sentences: The incoming word predictability effect. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29( 3), 260-273. 10.1080/01690965.2012.760745f7fde5e4893bbb63295264888ef9f1bdhttp%3A%2F%2Fwww.tandfonline.com%2Fdoi%2Ffull%2F10.1080%2F01690965.2012.760745http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/01690965.2012.760745
[8]
Olsen R, Chiew M, Buchsbaum B, Ryan J ( 2014) Delay period eye movements support successful visuospatial memory binding. Journal of Vision 14, 1-11.
[9]
Johansson R, Johansson M ( 2014) Look here, eye movements play a functional role in memory retrieval. Psychological Science 25( 1), 236-242. 10.1177/09567976134982602416685697bc86f8724e6c6c89a80c9906b3ecafhttp%3A%2F%2Feuropepmc.org%2Fabstract%2Fmed%2F24166856http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0956797613498260
[10]
Esterman M, Yantis S ( 2009) Perceptual expectation evokes categoryselective cortical activity. Cerebral Cortex 20( 5), 1245-1253. 10.1093/cercor/bhp188285250944bc328e52bc980d3cd3c246626b65c0http%3A%2F%2Fcercor.oxfordjournals.org%2Fcontent%2F20%2F5%2F1245.fullhttp://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/content/20/5/1245.full
[11]
Lupyan G, Spivey MJ ( 2010) Making the invisible visible: Verbal but not visual cues enhance visual detection. Plos One 5( 7), e11452. 10.1371/journal.pone.001145243a063369347bc2b5e92be9686e900cbhttp%3A%2F%2Fwww.plosone.org%2Farticle%2Finfo%253Adoi%252F10.1371%252Fjournal.pone.0011452https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0011452
[12]
Bressler SL, Tang W, Sylvester CM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M ( 2008) Top-down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience 28( 40), 10056-10061. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1776-08.200818829963bfb7badb80591b210f8900b376bfe3fehttp%3A%2F%2Feuropepmc.org%2Farticles%2FPMC2583122%2Fhttp://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1776-08.2008
[13]
Silver MA, Ress D, Heeger DJ ( 2007) Neural correlates of sustained spatial attention in human early visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 97( 1), 229-237. 10.1152/jn.00677.200618685024d295ea7c814f5ea3e4d86ebc3018475http%3A%2F%2Feuropepmc.org%2Farticles%2FPMC1868502%2Fhttp://www.physiology.org/doi/10.1152/jn.00677.2006
[14]
Bar M ( 2003) A cortical mechanism for triggering top-down facilitation in visual object recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15( 4), 600-609. 10.1162/08989290332166297612803970891699e2b4c7e2ee33fbbab20bb4f793http%3A%2F%2Fieeexplore.ieee.org%2Fxpls%2Fabs_all.jsp%3Farnumber%3D6789603http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/10.1162/089892903321662976
[15]
Everling S, Johnston K ( 2013) Control of the superior colliculus by the lateral prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 368( 1628), 20130068. 10.1098/rstb.2013.006837582106fe646ffdc2b31f6c0c3c04447a67d02http%3A%2F%2Fwww.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov%2Fpmc%2Farticles%2FPMC3758210%2Fhttp://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/cgi/doi/10.1098/rstb.2013.0068
[16]
Carello CD, Krauzlis RJ ( 2004) Manipulating intent: evidence for a causal role of the superior colliculus in target selection. Neuron 43( 4), 575-583. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.07.026http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896627304004672
[17]
McPeek RM, Keller EL ( 2004) Deficits in saccade target selection after inactivation of superior colliculus. Nature Neuroscience 7( 7), 757-763. 10.1038/nn1269151950996a9711b5c5cd031754f56711129a0dbahttp%3A%2F%2Feuropepmc.org%2Fabstract%2FMED%2F15195099http://www.nature.com/articles/nn1269
[18]
Hafed ZM, Goffart L, Krauzlis RJ ( 2009) A neural mechanism for microsaccade generation in the primate superior colliculus. Science 323( 5916), 940-943. 10.1126/science.116611226551185ec5b70f3fbb42599a3d2e57ab93a590http%3A%2F%2Fpubmedcentralcanada.ca%2Fpmcc%2Farticles%2FPMC2655118%2Fhttp://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/doi/10.1126/science.1166112
[19]
Hafed ZM, Krauzlis RJ ( 2012) Similarity of superior colliculus involvement in microsaccade and saccade generation. Journal of Neurophysiology 107( 7), 1904-1916. 10.1152/jn.01125.2011f8e66fc3d5ec08e181f9febe55fd1f86http%3A%2F%2Fdev.europepmc.org%2Fabstract%2Fmed%2F22236714%2Freload%3D2%3Bjsessionid%3Dwhsj9gjly6mlogypne5v.0http://www.physiology.org/doi/10.1152/jn.01125.2011
[20]
Piras A, Raffi M, Lanzoni IM, Persiani M, Squatrito S ( 2015) Microsaccades and prediction of a motor act outcome in a dynamic sport situation. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 56( 8), 4520-4530. 10.1167/iovs.15-16880262004900e4c4f9f92cbaa1b1daf208d77a12462http%3A%2F%2Feuropepmc.org%2Fabstract%2FMED%2F26200490http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/26200490
[21]
Kliegl R, Nuthmann A, Engbert R ( 2006) Tracking the mind during reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 135( 1), 12-35. 10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.121647831455575287e3bff46a156c22ab9d734184http%3A%2F%2Feuropepmc.org%2Fabstract%2FMED%2F16478314http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.12
[22]
Engbert R, Kliegl R ( 2003) Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research 43( 9), 1035-1045. 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00084-112676246c305c14c9058f2b475bb86be53013011http%3A%2F%2Fwww.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov%2Fpubmed%2F12676246http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0042698903000841
[23]
Martinez-Conde S, Macknik SL, Hubel DH ( 2004) The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5( 3), 229-240. 10.1038/nrn134814976522459cbebf7bdf490632487d7281424b07http%3A%2F%2Fwww.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov%2Fpubmed%2F14976522%3Fdopt%3DAbstracthttp://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14976522?dopt=Abstract
[24]
Summerfield C, Egner T ( 2009) Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13( 9), 403-409. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.00319716752aab1195899a7f2fc163ad6d778ef6c08http%3A%2F%2Fwww.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov%2Fpubmed%2F19716752https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1364661309001442
[25]
Puri AM, Wojciulik E, Ranganath C ( 2009) Category expectation modulates baseline and stimulus-evoked activity in human inferotemporal cortex. Brain Research 1301, 89-99. 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.085197474638da65d5f64b89320e414a143e2cb8a8fhttp%3A%2F%2Fpsycnet.apa.org%2Fpsycinfo%2F2009-18033-001https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0006899309018599
[26]
Moscovitch M ( 2008) The hippocampus as a “stupid”, domain-specific module: Implications for theories of recent and remote memory, and of imagination. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale 62( 1), 62-79. 10.1037/1196-1961.62.1.6211461708af8b4fff69ce9e2d77a034b1http%3A%2F%2Fbrain.oxfordjournals.org%2Flookup%2Fexternal-ref%3Faccess_num%3D18473631%26amp%3Blink_type%3DMED%26amp%3Batom%3D%252Fbrain%252F137%252F12%252F3355.atomhttp://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/1196-1961.62.1.62
[27]
Kennedy A, Pynte J, Murray WS, Paul SA ( 2013) Frequency and predictability effects in the Dundee Corpus: An eye movement analysis. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 66( 3), 601-618. 10.1080/17470218.2012.6760542264311885d1f7a68eedd502dd033639a67cd5d5http%3A%2F%2Feuropepmc.org%2Fabstract%2FMED%2F22643118http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1080/17470218.2012.676054
[28]
Cummings JL, Houlihan JP, Hill MA ( 1986) The pattern of reading deterioration in dementia of the Alzheimer type: Observations and implications. Brain and Language 29( 2), 315-323. 10.1016/0093-934X(86)90051-93790984e2ba04a0d5656c6e6dfe6d0cf6d38c28http%3A%2F%2Fwww.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov%2Fpubmed%2F3790984http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0093934X86900519
[29]
Fernández G, Manes F, Rotstein NP, Colombo O, Mandolesi P, Politi LE, Agamennoni O ( 2014) Lack of contextual-word predictability during reading in patients with mild Alzheimer disease. Neuropsychologia 62, 143-151. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.02325080188b1106952bf799c41b7e232dc87cdfc9ahttp%3A%2F%2Fwww.sciencedirect.com%2Fscience%2Farticle%2Fpii%2FS0028393214002437https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393214002437
Share
Back to top